Our brains are constantly interpreting incomplete and ambiguous sensory information about the environment (Helmholtz,
1924; Wolfe,
1996; Andrews & Purves,
1997). The phenomenon of binocular rivalry (BR) has long been used to investigate the mechanisms by which conflicting interpretations of sensory data are resolved (Wheatstone,
1838; for reviews see Blake & Logothetis,
2002; Alais & Blake,
in press). To generate BR, dissimilar stimuli are presented to the two eyes, such that they fall on corresponding regions of the two retinas. Although the two physical stimuli do not change, awareness switches spontaneously between them. Thus, as with other bistable stimuli, such as the Necker cube (Necker,
1832), multistable apparent motion (Ramachandran & Anstis,
1985), and the face-vase (Rubin,
1958), BR serves to decouple changes in perceptual awareness from changes in stimulation.
Here we investigated perceptual stability across interruptions in two types of rivalry, classical BR and stimulus rivalry (SR). In the SR paradigm, the images presented to the two eyes are swapped rapidly, yet observers experience one image-dominating perception for 3–4 eye-swaps (Logothetis, Leopold, & Sheinberg
1996). Because the images presented to the two eyes are continually swapping at a rate much faster than the perceptual one, it follows that it cannot be monocular information that is rivaling. However, it has been shown that dominance phase durations for BR and SR follow similar distributions and exhibit a lack of temporal relation between each phase, providing strong evidence that the two kinds of rivalry involve similar mechanisms.
The conditions producing SR seem narrower than those leading to BR, hence it has been suggested that they arise from different (albeit similar) mechanisms (Lee & Blake,
1999). Indeed, it has been widely argued that rivalry can occur at multiple levels of the visual hierarchy (Blake & Logothetis,
2002; Alais & Blake,
in press; Lee & Blake,
1999; Andrews,
2001; Bonneh, Sagi, & Karni,
2001; Tong,
2001; Freeman & Nguyen,
2001).
Orbach, Ehrlich, and Heath (
1963) demonstrated that intermittent presentation of the Necker cube brings its perceptual alternations almost to a standstill. It has recently been reported that if rivalrous gratings disappear for a short period, the stimulus in awareness as they disappear tends to be the one perceived when they reappear (Leopold, Wilke, Maier, & Logothetis,
2002). Thus, if a blank screen interrupts rivalry, perceptual alternations can be brought almost to a standstill. It has been suggested that a form of perceptual “memory” is responsible for storing the perceptual configuration of the stimulus during the blank interruption (Leopold et al.,
2002; Maier, Wilke, Logothetis, & Leopold,
2003), and that this memory seems to be dependent on location in the visual field (Blake, Sobel, & Gilroy,
2003). However, the details of this mnemonic mechanism and its relationship to changes in awareness during rivalry are poorly understood.
Blake, Westendorf, and Overton (
1980) demonstrated that when dominant and suppressed patterns are interchanged between the eyes during BR, observers’ percept switched to that of the previously suppressed pattern. This suggests that during continuously viewed rivalry it is an entire eye’s image that is suppressed, and that this suppression remains constant despite changes in stimulation. If this is the case, it may be the dominant eye that is “remembered” across a blank gap in BR. Given that the eye from which an image is sourced is not available to awareness (Ono & Barbeito,
1985), this would be qualitatively different to a memory for the previous percept per se.
In Experiment 1, we investigated the contents of perceptual memory across interruptions in BR. We presented subjects with a pair of gratings of opposite color and orientation, followed by a blank interruption. During the interruption, we swapped either the color, orientation, or eye of presentation of the gratings. Comparing the percepts reported before and after the interruption allowed us to establish what aspects of perception remained stable. The data showed that the most stable aspect of perception across the interruption was the eye-of-origin of the perceived stimulus, although dominant color and orientation also remained stable at rates significantly higher than chance. A strong effect of eye-of-origin on perceptual stability across a blank interval during BR was also reported recently at Vision Sciences (Chen & He,
2003).
In Experiment 2, we eliminated eye-of-origin information by switching the rivalrous stimuli between the two eyes 4 times/s to produce SR (Logothetis, et al.,
1996). Under these conditions of SR, color was the most stable attribute. The findings from the two experiments suggest that the mnemonic mechanisms responsible for the stable perception of rivalry are mediated at multiple levels of the visual hierarchy, contingent on the stimulus parameters, much like the mechanisms mediating rivalry itself.