January 2002
Volume 2, Issue 1
Free
Research Article  |   January 2002
Classification images: A tool to analyze visual strategies
Author Affiliations
Journal of Vision January 2002, Vol.2, i. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/2.1.i
  • Views
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Miguel P. Eckstein, Albert J. Ahumada; Classification images: A tool to analyze visual strategies. Journal of Vision 2002;2(1):i. https://doi.org/10.1167/2.1.i.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Introduction
Every year experimental techniques appear or resurface that provide tools and theoretical frameworks with which to study the mechanisms and processes involved in human visual perception. Occasionally, one of these techniques will resonate and trigger a wave of excitement among a group of researchers in the field, and will lead to a large number of studies using the method. This was so in the case of the technique known as classification images which was first presented at the European Conference on Visual Perception (Ahumada, 1996). 
The technique has still earlier roots in auditory work (Ahumada & Lovell, 1971), wherein multiple regression analysis was applied to the problem of auditory tone detection in noise, to estimate the contribution of auditory stimulus features to the observer’s decision variable. The regression weights plotted as a function of feature temporal frequency could be called classification plots. The central concept of the technique is the correlation of observer decisions with noisy stimulus features over sets of stimuli. From the correlation of the features with the decisions and the inter-correlations among the features, the investigator can then estimate how the observer is weighting the stimulus features to reach a decision. 
In the 1980’s and 1990’s a related technique, reverse correlation, was used to estimate the features of receptive fields of the visual cortex (e.g., Jones & Palmer, 1987; Ohzawa, DeAngelis, & Freeman, 1996; Ringach, Hawken, & Shapley 1997). 
Ahumada and Beard applied the technique in visual psychophysics to study vernier acuity tasks (Ahumada, 1996; Beard & Ahumada, 1998). They used image pixel intensity as the stimulus features, so that the stimulus feature weights did form an actual classification image. The study revealed that human observers weighted the visual information of the stimuli differently from the optimum (ideal) observer. However, perhaps more importantly, it provided a first glance at the technique for other scientists, and sparked their imagination about the many questions that might be addressed using the technique. Could it be used to study stereo, illusory contours, letter discrimination and perceptual learning? Could it be used with tasks other than the yes/no task? What would be the best (most efficient) algorithm to estimate the classification images? 
Why do classification images appeal to vision scientists? Is it the visual nature of the classification image, or is it its exploratory nature that resembles a sort of archeology of perceptual processes? for whatever reason, soon after 1996 a number of researchers started using the technique to study a variety of problems in human visual perception including letter discrimination (Watson & Rosenholtz, 1997), perceptual learning (Knoblauch, Thomas, & D’Zmura, 1999), illusory contours (Gold, Murray, Bennett, & Sekuler, 2000), foveal versus peripheral vernier performance (Beard & Ahumada, 1999), stereo (Neri, Parker, & Blakemore, 1999), and off-frequency looking in non-white noise (Abbey & Eckstein, 2000). 
On the other hand, some researchers have watched with skepticism from the sidelines the development and use of the classification image technique. Can one really know whether an empirical classification image is meaningful or simply noise? Can the experimenter’s interpretation of the classification image be biased so that he/she simply sees confirmatory evidence of a hypothesis? How can one reach any meaningful conclusions when comparing two classification images? What are the underlying assumptions of the technique and how meaningful is the technique when the assumptions are violated? All of these questions should have clear answers for a mature scientific method, and should oblige investigators to use scientific rigor in the use of the technique. These questions have indeed motivated investigators to develop sound procedures to estimate the classification image and to test different hypotheses about the obtained classification images (e.g. Is the classification image statistically significantly different from noise? Is it significantly different from the classification image of the ideal observer?) 
The papers in this special issue of the Journal of Vision describe methods to estimate classification images and to test hypotheses about them. The papers also provide examples of the wide variety of topics and questions in visual perception that can be addressed using the technique. As guest editors for this special issue, we hope it will provide a useful starting point for researchers interested in embarking in projects using this novel and exciting technique. 
References
Abbey, C. K. Eckstein, M. P. (2000). Estimates of human-observer templates for simple detection tasks in correlated noise. Proceedings of the SPIE, 3981, 70–77.
Ahumada, A. J.Jr. (1996). Perceptual classification images from Vernier acuity masked by noise [Abstract]. Perception, 26, 18. [Link]
Ahumada, A. J.Jr. Lovell, J. (1971). Stimulus features in signal detection. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 49, 1751–1756. [CrossRef]
Beard, B. L. Ahumada, A. J.Jr. (1998). A technique to extract relevant image features for visual tasks. SPIE Proceedings, 3299, 79–85.
Beard, B. L. Ahumada, A. J.Jr. (1999). Detection in fixed and random noise in foveal and parafoveal vision explained by template learning. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 16(3), 755–763. [PubMed] [CrossRef]
Gold, J. M. Murray, R. F. Bennett, P. J. Sekuler, A. B. (2000). Deriving behavioural receptive fields for visually completed contours. Current Biology, 10, 663–666. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Jones, J. P. Palmer, L. A. (1987). The two-dimensional spatial structure of simple receptive fields in cat striate cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 58, 1187–1211. [PubMed] [PubMed]
Knoblauch, K. Thomas, J. P. DrsZmura, M. (1999). Feedback temporal frequency and stimulus classification [Abstract]. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 40(4), 4171.
Neri, P. Parker, A. J. Blakemore, C. (1999). Probing the human stereoscopic system with reverse correlation. Nature, 401, 695–698. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ohzawa, I. DeAngelis, G. C. Freeman, R. D. (1996). Encoding of binocular disparity by simple cells in the cat’s visual cortex. J Neurophysiol, 75(5), 1779–1805. [PubMed] [PubMed]
Ringach, D. L. Hawken, M. J. Shapley, R. (1997). Dynamics of orientation tuning in macaque primary visual cortex. Nature, 387(6630), 281–284.[PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Watson, A. B. Rosenholtz, R. (1997). A Rorschach test for visual classification strategies [Abstract]. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 38(4), 2.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×