The results show that task (trial) played a more dominant role compared to subject and his/her individual strategies (see
Figure 4). The NSS coherence for the four possible comparison types was analyzed using repeated measures ANOVA with comparison type as the single categorical predictor and observations matched for subject and trial combinations. Three planned contrasts were performed to evaluate the effect of task/trial (SSST vs. SSDT), the effect of interpersonal differences (SSST vs. DSST), and whether coherence in the same subject across different trials differed from baseline (SSDT vs. DSDT).
The analysis showed that coherence depended on comparison type, F(3, 477) = 835.15, p < 0.001, ηG2 = 0.754. As expected, the greatest amount of coherence was found in intra-individual performance while watching identical trials repeatedly (SSST: M = 3.89, SD = 1.19). The second greatest amount of coherence was found in different subjects while watching the same trials (DSST: M = 2.86, SD = 0.91). The difference between coherence in SSST and DSST was significant, t(159) = 12.96, p < 0.001, Cohen d = 0.97 ± 0.23, which means that the interindividual coherence within a trial was significantly lower than the intra-individual coherence (diff = 1.03). Coherence in the same subject watching different trials was much lower (SSDT: M = 0.66, SD = 0.47) and significantly different from coherence in the SSST comparison, diff = 3.23, t(159) = 34.02, p < 0.001, d = 3.57 ± 0.35. SSDT coherence did not differ significantly from the baseline, diff = 0.06, t(159) = 1.46, p = 0.145, d = 0.14 ± 0.22; baseline DSDT: M = 0.60; SD = 0.42. The lack of significance may indicate that in the current experimental conditions, subjects actually tracked objects using eye movements; if subjects fixated on the center of the screen and tracked the targets using only their visual attention, eye-movement coherence of unrelated trials would be higher.
I also analyzed the intra-individual variability of the NSS coherence across trials. I limited the scope of the analysis to the SSST and analyzed the differences in two separate one-way ANOVAs. The first ANOVA used subject as a categorical predictor, and the second ANOVA used trial id as a categorical predictor (with 16 levels). I found significant differences in intra-individual coherence across subjects ranging from 1.65 to 5.37, M = 3.89, SD = 0.92, F(19, 140) = 9.702, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.568. In other words, some subjects were more likely to repeat a pattern of their eye movements than other subjects. The differences in intra-individual coherence between different trials were not significant, F(15, 144) = 1.061, p = 0.398, η2 = 0.100. A significant result would mean that some trials elicit more coherent eye movements than others (given the selection of trials in the present experiment).
I examined whether the eye movements of different people became more similar or diverse over time. The mean NSS value from the DSST comparison for each block ranged from 3.04 (Block 1) to 3.38 (Block 2). To estimate the effect of time I averaged the corresponding NSS values from the first two blocks and last two blocks and compared them with paired t test. No significant changes in interindividual eye-movement similarity were observed over time, t(31) = 0.541, p = 0.593, d = 0.07 ± 0.49.
In order to find out whether the repetition affects the type of eye movements, I analyzed the changes in number of saccades, total time occupied by them, and their median length in both repeating and unique trials. During average trial participants made 9.86 saccades (SD = 5.58) with median length 3.24° (SD = 1.35), which occupied 330 ms (SD = 220) of total 8 s. To estimate the effect of time I grouped the data from the first two blocks and last two blocks and used repeated measures ANOVA with two factors (condition: repeating/unique, and time). I found a significant decrease in number of saccades over time, F(1, 19) = 5.178, p = 0.035, ηG2 = 0.010, no effect of condition, F(1, 19) = 1.689, p = 0.209, ηG2 = 0.001, and significant interaction, F(1, 19) = 7.092, p = 0.015, ηG2 = 0.002, showing the number of saccades decreases faster in unique trials. For the total time occupied by saccades there was a significant interaction, F(1, 19) = 5.644, p = 0.028, ηG2 = 0.001, with no effect of time, F(1, 19) = 3.727, p = 0.069, ηG2 = 0.007, or condition, F(1, 19) = 1.336, p = 0.262, ηG2 = 0.001. There was no significant effect for median saccade length, time: F(1, 19) = 0.460, p = 0.501, ηG2 = 0.002; condition: F(1, 19) = 2.746, p = 0.114, ηG2 = 0.008; interaction: F(1, 19) = 1.503, p = 0.235, ηG2 = 0.002. The results show a decrease in number of saccades during experiment, which was larger in unique trials. This decrease is partly mirrored in less total time occupied by saccades. The length of saccades did not change over time.
In SSST comparison incorrect trials were included to retain the number of trials in each NSS calculation and keep the results comparable. I compared the NSS values for SSST comparisons with and without any error. The majority of comparisons contained no error (122 of 160) and yielded slightly higher NSS values, M = 3.99, SD = 1.18, with errors: M = 3.56, SD = 1.20, t(60.6) = 1.945, p = 0.056, d = 0.37 ± 0.37. Therefore adding incorrect trials lowered NSS coherence in SSST approximately by 0.1.
To provide additional insight into NSS values and their relationship to actual gaze positions, I repeated the analysis using a different approach based on gaze distances. I used the same comparisons as in the previous analysis, but I compared scan patterns using their mutual distances in every frame. For each frame (85 Hz) I calculated all distances between scan patterns involved in the comparison. Because in each frame several gaze samples (up to three) were present, I included only the first sample in the analysis. Then I calculated the mean, median, minimum, and maximum distance in each frame and the median values of these parameters over all frames in each trial. The following results are based on the analysis of mean distances, but all four parameters were highly correlated and yielded similar results.
The analysis based on gaze distances confirmed the results based on NSS (see
Figure 5a). In SSST comparison the mean gaze distance was 2.51° (
SD = 1.07), which was significantly smaller than in DSST comparison,
M = 3.09,
SD = 0.71,
t(159) = 12.117,
p < 0.001, Cohen
d = 0.93 ± 0.23. The mean gaze distances in SSDT comparison were not significantly different from DSDT condition,
M = 5.75,
SD = 0.95,
t(159) = 1.324,
p = 0.188,
d = 0.12 ± 0.22, and significantly larger than in SSST comparison,
t(159) = 32.927,
p < 0.001,
d = 3.45 ± 0.35.
Figure 5b shows the relationship between mean gaze distance and NSS measures. Despite differences in calculations and the limitations discussed later, both measures show good fit—NSS varied logarithmically with mean gaze distance (
R2 = 0.885 ± 0.017).