In what follows, we investigate how chromatic and achromatic cues combine to determine the reliability or precision of perceived transparency. Although there has been a great deal of research on both chromatic and achromatic transparency (Beck et al.,
1984; Da Pos,
1989; D'Zmura et al.,
1997; Faul & Ekroll,
2002; Gerbino et al.,
1990; Hagedorn & D'Zmura,
2000; Khang & Zaidi,
2002; Nakuchi, Silfsten, Parkkinen, & Ussui,
1999; Singh,
2004; Singh & Anderson,
2002), the question of how these two sources of information combine to determine percepts of transparency has not been addressed. Physical processes such as colored filters typically confound changes in luminance with changes in chroma. We will manipulate the chromatic and achromatic properties of our displays independently to avoid such confounds.
The primary measure that we will use is the
reliability, ρ, of observers' settings for the adjustable sector, defined as the reciprocal of setting variance (Backus & Banks,
1999). We specifically investigate whether the
combination of chromatic and achromatic cues leads to a more reliable percept of transparency. For
Figure 3A, we noted that in a six-region achromatic display, the convergence model predicts a unique luminance
f1 for the variable sector as demonstrated in the diagram in
Figure 3B. In an experiment using this configuration, Kasrai and Kingdom (
2001) found that although this luminance prediction for the variable sector is supported, there is in fact a relatively wide range of luminance values around
f1 that generate a percept of transparency. We ask whether the addition of equiluminant color to such a display will “sharpen” the percept of transparency, thereby decreasing the variability in observers' settings.
In addition to the achromatic case—which we will refer to as the
L condition—we will have a purely chromatic (equiluminant color) condition,
C. An example of such a condition is shown in
Figure 4A, in which all chromatic variations are defined along the single dimension of saturation—going from neutral to highly saturated yellow. We emphasize that, in the chromatic displays, the six regions are equiluminant. It is clear that, as in the achromatic case, there is a unique solution (here, for the saturation of the adjustable sector) that satisfies the convergence model (depicted in
Figure 4B). A “cue-combined” version will be created by superimposing the
L and
C displays, so that the transparent filter in the resulting
L +
C display contains both achromatic and chromatic convergence. The details of how the superposition was achieved will be explained in the
General methods section. The critical question will be whether setting reliability in the
L +
C case is systematically higher than in the
L case. Moreover, to address the question of whether the introduction of
any colors (rather than just polarity-preserving, transparency-consistent colors) improves the percept of transparency, we included a fourth condition,
L +
iC, in which the chromatic component of the superposition is polarity reversed—hence, inconsistent with transparency (recall Metelli's qualitative constraints).
The first two experiments test whether adding color to luminance information makes transparency more precise relative to either cue in isolation. The third experiment applies further configuration manipulations to determine whether the relative strength and contrast between the two cues influence perceived transparency.