Reinforcement learning provides a more general mechanism to account for these sensorimotor changes than these specialized motor calibration mechanisms. We have previously proposed that the oculomotor system could increase the probability of saccades that had a better value, i.e., those that land close to target, while avoiding the ones that had lower value by manipulating the postsaccadic reinforcement in the absence of a position error signal (Madelain, Paeye, & Wallman,
2011). In the present experiment the value of saccades is clearly related to the distance from the target and not to the distance from background features. Stated otherwise, saccades were reinforced by vision of the target, not by vision of the background. A similar conclusion was reached when using a postsaccadic target and distractor to probe adaptation (Madelain et al.,
2010). The sensitivity of saccades to reinforcement is compatible with the observation that when monkeys had to perform a saccade in a direction associated with food, saccade peak velocities were higher, trajectories straighter and latencies shorter than in a nonreinforced direction (Lauwereyns, Watanabe, Coe, & Hikosaka,
2002; Takikawa, Kawagoe, Itoh, Nakahara, & Hikosaka,
2002; Watanabe, Lauwereyns, & Hikosaka,
2003). In humans reinforcement has been shown to affect several saccade properties (Madelain, Paeye & Darcheville,
2011) such as peak velocities and durations (Xu-Wilson, Zee, & Shadmehr,
2009), variability in saccade endpoints (Paeye & Madelain,
2011), saccade gain (Madelain, Paeye, & Wallman,
2011), or saccade latencies (Madelain, Champrenaut, & Chauvin,
2007). That saccade adaptation is selectively driven by the saccade goal can be interpreted as further evidence of the saccadic system's sensitivity to reinforcement.