One way for the brain to obtain an estimate of 3-D motion is to obtain independent measures of the
X-component and the
Z-component. Cumming & Parker (
1994) suggested how the
Z-component may be obtained, and Beverley & Regan (
1975) demonstrated that 3-D motion trajectories can be precisely discriminated. There is evidence, from motion threshold studies, that there may be two independent mechanisms for 3-D motion detection (Sumnall & Harris,
2002), although the
Z-component mechanism appears to be much less sensitive (Harris, McKee, & Watamaniuk,
1998; Sumnall & Harris,
2000; Tyler,
1971). If 3-D motion perception relied on a combination of independently obtained
X-components and
Z-components of motion, one might expect perception of 3-D motion direction to be biased during eye movements. It is known that the
X-component is likely to be slightly biased during
X-direction pursuit due to incomplete eye movement compensation. No studies have previously directly measured how the
Z-component might be biased due to vergence. However, there are suggestions that perception is likely to be biased. When vergence is the only cue to the presence of
Z-motion,
Z-motion is not perceived over a range of speeds, distances, and stimulus sizes (Brenner, Van Den Berg, & Van Damme,
1996; Erkelens & Collewijn,
1985a; Regan, Erkelens, & Collewijn,
1986). This suggests that the extraretinal signal for motion-in-depth associated with vergence eye movements is weak if not absent, but note that there is evidence for the use of extraretinal vergence signals for other tasks. For example, changes in vergence can be used to partially scale disparity for perceived curvature (Rogers & Bradshaw,
1995; Watt, Akeley, Ernst, & Banks,
2005). Vergence is also thought to be involved in the scaling of object size: For example, afterimages can change size when vergence is changed (Mon-Williams, Tresilian, Plooy, Wann, & Broerse,
1997). However, some of the studies suggesting poor extraretinal information from vergence used stimuli containing conflicting cues. In particular, some used large stimuli that changed their disparity while their size was held constant; hence, there were cues to change in depth from disparity, alongside cues to no change in depth from constant size. Here, I used small, impoverished stimuli that reduce the salience of the no-motion size cue (Gray & Regan,
1999).