Figure 2 shows the proportion of two directions reported for red while varying the amount of red presented in the second direction (data pooled over all participants). The mean of the distribution indicates how long red must be shown along the second direction for observers to report two directions for red color in 50% of trials.
When the last green color interval was absent we found a mean of 55 ms for the 180° direction change (distribution of triangles in
Figure 2) and a mean of 19 ms for the 90° direction change (distribution of diamonds in
Figure 2). These results show that when the dots move along the second direction for very short times, this direction of motion is not perceived. Interestingly, the mean is significantly reduced for the direction change of 90° with respect to the 180° condition (
p < .001) and in a magnitude very similar to that reported in
Experiment 1. This suggests that the perceptual delay in motion perception, as measured in
Experiment 1, reflects that motion signal needs integration time to reach awareness (Burr & Santoro,
2001) and that this time depends on the former direction of motion. As the maximum asynchrony is obtained when reversing the direction, we think, as stated elsewhere (Arnold & Clifford,
2002; Bedell et al.,
2003; Clifford et al.,
2004), that mechanisms of opponency in MT underlie this perceptual asynchrony (Snowden, Treue, Erickson, & Andersen,
1991).
When the last green interval was present, the red dots had to be shown moving along the second direction for a slightly longer time (mean of 73 ms) to be equally perceived moving in one or two directions in comparison to when green was absent (
p < .001 for the 180° condition distribution of squares in
Figure 2). For the 90° condition (distribution of circles in
Figure 2), this difference was not significant. In agreement with an effect of the last green interval, one would expect larger differences in the PSE (e.g., curve shifted further to the right when the green is present). We think that the lack of such an effect might have been caused by a response bias that shifts the curve to the left. The fact that subjects responded in several trials “two directions” for the relative timing of 0 for which red only is presented in one direction may reflect this bias. In any case, it is clear that the last green interval has an effect on the visibility of the red interval, which is shown by the significant variation of the slopes for the two angle conditions. The standard deviation increased from 25 to 97 ms for the 180° condition and from 11 to 64 ms for the 90° condition presumably reflecting a higher difficulty of the task.
Our findings share similarities with previous results published by Moradi and Shimojo (
2004). They also reported a misbinding of color and motion within a single event. In one condition of their Experiment 5, observers were asked to report the color of a briefly moving surface that became perceptually segregated. During motion, the dots inside this surface were gray and the color switched back to green when they stopped. Subjects reported the color following motion offset more often than gray, which was the actual color during motion. Moradi and Shimojo regarded this result as the onset of a new surface triggering the analysis of the properties (including color) of the surface. They suggested that these properties are computed during a temporal window of 50–150 ms following the onset of the surface. Remarkably, the gray color was not perceived even when presented for 120 ms. As the temporal window of analysis lasts for 150 ms at the most, this implies that the color is not treated uniformly during the time window of analysis (Arnold,
2005) but it is temporally weighted distinctly favoring color information available later in time.
We think that another, and maybe simpler, explanation of the effect of the last color interval could be backward masking (Bachman,
1994). According to it, the last green color interval would mask red color in the last part of the red color interval. This account does not require any direction change to work. Thus, regardless of the direction change, a fixed quantity of red color would be masked. Although, it may seem contradictory, we think this is consistent with the fact that the difference (
p < .001) between the two direction changes (90° and 180°) has the same trend as when the last color interval is absent: the last green color interval could mask a physically longer interval of red presented after a direction change of 180° simply because the first part of motion is not perceived in this case (last interval absent condition).
In summary, taking into account the results of
Experiments 1 and
2, we believe that in a typical cycle display with several alternations, the perceived asynchrony genuinely reflects differential processing latencies between color and motion: perceptual experience of motion is delayed when motion in the opposite direction is previously displayed. However, we show that some form of visual masking also contributes to the percept making the perception of color after direction changes more difficult and facilitating the pairing of colors with directions presented before direction changes.