Human performance differs at different locations in the visual field. In addition to the performance reduction for visual locations in the periphery farther from the center of gaze (DeValois & DeValois,
1988; Duncan & Boynton,
2003), the lower visual field (below fixation) supports better performance than the upper visual field, even at the same eccentricity (Altpeter, Mackeben, & Trauzettel-Klosinski,
2000; Edgar & Smith,
1990; He, Cavanagh, & Intriligator,
1996; Levine & McAnany,
2005; Previc,
1990; Rubin, Nakayama, & Shapley,
1996). A series of studies report that this performance asymmetry in many tasks is restricted to the vertical meridian: Performance is worse along the upper than along the lower region of the vertical meridian but shows no difference between upper and lower visual fields for nonmeridian locations (Cameron, Tai, & Carrasco,
2002; Carrasco, Giordano, & McElree,
2004; Carrasco, Talgar, & Cameron,
2001; Carrasco, Williams, & Yeshurun,
2002; Talgar & Carrasco,
2002). Hence, we have referred to this phenomenon as vertical meridian asymmetry (VMA). The VMA becomes more pronounced with increasing spatial frequency—it is barely present for low-spatial-frequency Gabor stimuli and gradually becomes more pronounced for intermediate and high frequencies (Cameron et al.,
2002; Carrasco et al.,
2001; Skrandies,
1987). This asymmetry has been observed in detection, discrimination, and localization tasks, in which performance is based on contrast sensitivity (Cameron et al.,
2002; Carrasco et al.,
2001), acuity (Carrasco et al.,
2002), and spatial resolution (Talgar & Carrasco,
2002). Here, we investigated the neural correlate of the VMA.