These shifts in stereo matching to the fixation plane are called an “illusion” because, for real wallpaper, the pattern on the paper floats freely in front of the wall, far from its veridical depth plane. However, from a physiological standpoint, the responses of stereo mechanisms to the illusory shifts may be identical to those generated by features physically presented in the fixation plane. Stereo thresholds could indicate whether a shifted grating produces the same neural response as a grating presented in the fixation plane. A threshold is a measure of the signal-to-noise ratio of the responsible neural pool, so identical thresholds would suggest that the same neural pool is functioning in both cases.
Our three test conditions are diagrammed in
Figure 8. Condition A is the standard configuration for measuring stereoacuity. The test target is a 3-cpd, 6°-wide grating segment presented in the fixation plane. The black probe line, presented below the lower edge of the grating, can appear in one of five possible disparities chosen at random from a narrow range. The observer judges whether the probe target is in front or behind the grating. In Condition B, the segment is presented at an uncrossed disparity of 20 arcmin. As in Condition A, the probe changes in small steps centered on the fixation plane, but now, the observer judges whether the distance separating the grating from the probe is smaller or larger than 20 arcmin, that is, the mean disparity separating the grating from the probe. This incremental judgment of relative disparity is more difficult than the standard stereoacuity judgment, but well-trained observers achieve stable, repeatable thresholds (McKee, Levi, & Bowne,
1990). The spatial arrangements for Condition C are identical to those for Condition B, but now, the stereoacuity judgment is made
after the apparent shift to the fixation plane.
The timing of the stimulus components is crucial for the success of this experiment. As shown by the diagrams on the right part of
Figure 8, the probe is visible only for the last 200 ms of any trial. Obviously, judgments about the relative disparity of the probe and grating can only be made when the probe is visible. However, if the fixation point remained visible throughout the trial, the observer might ignore the grating and base his or her judgments on the relative disparity of the probe with respect to the fixation point. To minimize this possibility for Conditions A and B, we turned off the fixation point when we presented the grating (
Figure 8D) and then waited 500 ms before presenting the probe stimulus. For Condition C, we turned off the fixation point when the observer signaled that the apparent depth of the grating had shifted forward to the fixation plane (
Figure 8E) and again waited 500 ms before presenting the probe. The observer might still try to make the judgment based on the previously visible fixation point because sequential judgments of relative disparity are possible (Enright,
1991; Frisby, Catherall, Porrill, & Buckley,
1997). Generally, this would be a bad strategy, because under our conditions (brief probe duration, memory-based judgment, etc.), sequential thresholds would be very poor, amounting to several minutes of arc (Enright,
1996). The important point is that after the fixation point disappears, the timing arrangements are identical for all three conditions. In all three, the probe appears 500 ms after the fixation point disappears, for 200 ms.
The results from this experiment are straightforward (
Figure 9). Predictably, thresholds for the grating segment in the fixation plane (Condition A) are quite good (∼14 arcsec). Thresholds for the segment presented 20 arcmin behind the fixation plane (Condition B) are poor, averaging 68 arcsec. However, if the observer waits until the illusory shift occurs (Condition C), the thresholds for the shifted grating are identical to the thresholds for Condition A. Without any change in the physical stimulus, the perceived shift in depth has improved thresholds by a factor of five.
Could these results be produced by convergence? The observer could easily converge to the grating plane during the long wait for the forward shift. However, this change in convergence would not improve stereoacuity thresholds. Both the grating and the probe have to be in or near the fixation plane to produce optimum stereoacuity. If the observer converges back to the grating plane, the probe will lie 20 arcmin in front of the grating. As shown by the difference in thresholds for Conditions A and B, a 20-arcmin standing disparity between probe and target degrades stereo sensitivity.
On the basis of these results, we conclude that the neural response associated with the illusory shift is identical to the response to a grating physically presented in the fixation plane. Once the envelope is adapted and loses its capacity to define the matching plane of the grating segment, the carrier is matched on the basis of the interocular phase disparity. Because the carrier is at zero phase for both the fixation plane and 20 arcmin, the precision of the response is the same for both.