Individual thresholds were estimated by linear interpolation to 75% correct. Interpolated vernier threshold (±1
SEM) was 54.4° of spatial phase (±4.5°) for infants, and 14.9° (±0.84°) for adults, a threshold ratio of 3.6:1. The similarity between infant and adult vernier performance was previously hidden in the literature by the confounding differences between infant and adult contrast sensitivity functions. When our results were converted into degrees of visual angle, vernier threshold was 0.378° v.a. for infants, and .007° v.a. for adults, a ratio of 55:1, which is closer to the standard values reviewed above. The infant vernier threshold value, in degrees of visual angle, was comparable to those reported in the literature (reviewed in the Introduction) when the size and contrast of the stimuli are taken into account. The adult vernier threshold value was comparable to those from the literature when the Gabor stimulus waveform is taken into account (Krauskopf & Farell,
1991). The difference between infant and adult vernier acuity was statistically highly significant by an unpaired
t test [
t12 = 8.69 (analyzed in deg of phase), and
t12 = 11.53 (analyzed in deg v.a.),
p < .0001 in each case].
CPCM thresholds can be expressed in two units. In Fourier-energy-equivalent phase-offset units, average infant CPCM threshold was 17.8° ± 1.08° of equivalent phase offset, and adult CPCM threshold was 7.03° ± 1.15° of equivalent phase offset, an infant:adult ratio of 2.53. In terms of contrast, recall that the higher contrast of the CPCM stimulus (panels
Figure 1 and
Figure 1 in
1994C) was always 1.0; the lower contrast (panels
10 and
d, d in
1994C), was 0.728 for infants, and 0.884 for adults, at the contrast discrimination threshold. The Weber fraction for contrast (ΔC/C) was 0.373 for infants and 0.131 for adults, a ratio of 2.85:1. This ratio is similar to the factor of 3 we have reported for contrast discrimination (Brown,
1994). The vernier:CPCM contrast threshold ratio was 3.05 for infants and 2.11 for adults. An analysis in log
10 units shows that the difference between them (0.159 l.u. ± 0.661) was not statistically significant.
Analyses of variance were performed on infant and adult data. In each case, the analysis considered three factors: subjects x stimulus type (CPCM vs. vernier) x (equivalent) phase offset. In each analysis, the factor “subjects” was not statistically significant, and did not interact significantly with any of the other factors. The infant analysis revealed that stimulus type was statistically significant (F1,10 =9 3.035, p < .00005), which is our main result: CPCM modulation was easier to see than vernier phase modulation. (Equivalent) phase offset (F2,20 = 47.49, p < .00005) was also significant, as stimuli were easier to see if the offsets or equivalent offsets were larger. And, stimulus type interacted significantly with offset, as the psychometric functions converged at higher (equivalent) offset values (stimulus type x phase offset: F2,20 = 8.197, p = .003). Similarly, the adult analysis revealed that stimulus type was statistically significant (F1,3 = 20.714, p = .02). (Equivalent) phase offset (F2,6 = 25.64, p = .001) was statistically significant, as was the interaction term (stimulus type x phase offset: (F2,6 = 12.395, p = .007). The vernier:CPCM threshold ratios were 2.8 for infants and 2.1 for adults. After converting them to logarithms and then doing a t test on their difference, t = 0.163, ns.
The statistical significance of stimulus type (CPCM vs. vernier) in both the infant and adult data sets indicates that the separation between the data and the major diagonal in
Figure 5 was statistically significant. Infant and adult vernier performance fell statistically significantly short of CPCM performance. This shows that neither infants nor adults performed the vernier task as well as the CPCM task, even though the Fourier energy components were identical in stimuli at equivalent offset values.