Most previous work on crowding used targets that differed from their background by a change in luminance, often referred to as
luminance-defined or
first-order targets. In the absence of a change in luminance, targets can still be distinguished from their background by a change in other stimulus attributes, such as contrast, texture, or motion. These stimuli are usually referred to as
second-order stimuli. There is abundant psychophysical evidence that suggests that the spatial and temporal properties of first- and second-order visual perception are different. For instance, although both first- and second-order mechanisms are tuned for orientation and spatial frequency of the visual stimuli, the tuning bandwidth is wider for second- than for first-order stimuli (Landy & Oruc,
2002). Unlike that for first-order stimuli, modulation contrast sensitivity for second-order stimuli demonstrates weak dependence on the spatial frequency of the second-order modulating pattern (Landy & Oruc,
2002). Further, adaptation to first-order stimuli shows spatial-frequency and orientation selectivity, whereas adaptation to second-order stimuli only shows spatial-frequency selectivity but transfers across orientations (McGraw, Levi, & Whitaker,
1999). The inhibitory effect due to the presence of flanking Gabors on a target Gabor shows higher specificity with respect to spatial frequency and orientation for first- than for second-order stimuli (Ellemberg, Allen, & Hess,
2004). The spatial extent of interaction between these Gabors extends over a larger distance for first- than for second-order stimuli (Ellemberg et al.,
2004), although this finding seems unexpected based on the larger receptive field sizes for neurons processing second-order information (Mareschal & Baker,
1998; Rosa,
1997). With respect to temporal properties, visual evoked potential latencies are shorter and psychophysical reaction times are faster for first- than for second-order motion stimuli (Ellemberg et al.,
2003). These results are in concordance with physiological (Baker & Mareschal,
2001; Mareschal & Baker,
1998) and brain-imaging (Dumoulin, Baker, Hess, & Evans,
2003; Larsson, Landy, & Heeger,
2006; Smith, Greenlee, Singh, Kraemer, & Hennig,
1998) evidence that there are two distinct processing streams for first- and second-order stimuli. Specifically, the processing of first-order information can be adequately represented by a linear filtering mechanism with a primary neural origin in V1. Processing of second-order information requires a nonlinear stage, and the current model for such processing is a filter–rectifier–filter mechanism, with possible neural substrate in V2 (Mareschal & Baker,
1998; Schofield,
2000).