Perception of biological motion is not just a single phenomenon. The perception of biological motion is composed of a rich palette of different aspects such as action recognition (Dittrich,
1993, Johansson,
1973; Pollick, Fidopiastis, & Braden,
2001), gender discrimination (Pollick, Lestou, Ryu, & Cho,
2002; Troje,
2002; Troje, Westhoff, & Lavrov,
2005), and identification of identity (Cutting & Kozlowski,
1977; Loula, Prasad, Harber, & Shiffrar,
2005). It has been shown that humans can use different information to judge movements depending on the task (Pollick et al.,
2001, Troje,
2002) and that the influence of bottom–up and top–down processing, as well as attention, differs among tasks (Thornton, Pinto, & Shiffrar,
1998; Thornton, Rensink, & Shiffrar,
2002; Thornton & Vuong,
2004). In this study, we have focused on straightforward discrimination tasks for simplicity. These simple tasks can be solved by a global form analysis in the absence of local motion signals. One may now ask: Which of the more complex aspects of biological motion perception are local and which ones are global? Which ones require motion per se and which ones are based on structural cues? In principle, a template model such as ours may be sufficient to also discriminate action, gender, or identity provided that the appropriate templates are available. The model arrives at a description of the temporal structure of the body posture change over time and, thus, may also discriminate actions and use dynamic cues (Troje,
2002) even if they are not derived from local motion analysis. Whether this is truly sufficient would have to be investigated in further studies; however, it is also likely that among the many aspects of biological motion, there are some that benefit from additional motion signals. However, for the task we studied here, these local motion signals do not form a critical feature for biological motion.