A stimulus feature is not a guiding attribute independent of context. For example, a red item will be found efficiently among green distractors but not among reddish orange distractors. The difference between target and distractor is critical (Duncan & Humphreys,
1989). A more curious aspect of the relationship of targets and distractors is that this relationship can be asymmetric. In some cases, the roles of target and distractor can be reversed without much consequence. It is easy to find red among green or green among red. In other cases, this is not true. For example, it is easier to find a moving target among stationary items than a stationary target among moving items (Dick,
1989; Royden, Wolfe, & Klempen,
2001). Treisman and Gormican (
1988) first suggested that search asymmetries could be a useful tool by noting that the presence of a feature can guide attention better than its absence. Thus, we could argue that motion is a feature and stationarity only its absence (but see Rosenholtz,
2001). Applying this search asymmetry logic, one would test search for A among B, and B among A. If one search produces shallow slopes and the other steeper slopes, then one could argue that the target producing shallow slopes bears the feature, and the other target is defined by the absence of the feature. Note that there are asymmetries where
both searches produce steep slopes but where one is markedly steeper than the other (e.g., search for upright among inverted faces, Tong & Nakayama,
1999). In such cases, neither property can be said to guide search. The difference in slopes is most likely a reflection of different rates of serial processing of items. Additionally, in some more complex cases, like detection of shadows, it is possible for the absence of a specific feature to alter the structure of a scene. In that case, it might be absence that is detected more readily than presence (Rensink & Cavanagh,
in press).