We found retinal image changes to be crucial to have a saccade cause a perceptual alternation for both free eye movements (
Experiment 1) and for fixation (
Experiment 2) and that retinal image changes without saccades can indeed determine percept dominance (
Experiment 3).
Although the experiments reported here were conducted using a relatively low spatial frequency, it is very likely that the results extend to binocular rivalry stimuli in general. A high positive correlation between saccades and perceptual alternations has already been reported for two other binocular rivalry stimuli: one consisting of 2.9 cycles/deg gratings and one consisting of an image of a house and an image of a face (van Dam & van Ee,
2006). It is unlikely that the previously found correlation had some other origin than the correlation reported here. Monocular effects like perceptual fading are also unlikely to have affected the results in a significant manner because similar results are found when square-wave gratings (for which monocular fading effects should be much reduced; Atkinson et al.,
1973; Campbell & Howell,
1972; Tulunay-Keesey,
1982) are used instead of sine-wave gratings.
The findings reported here are also consistent with previous reports that abrupt changes in one eye's stimulus (i.e., either phase or frequency changes of a grating; Walker & Powell,
1979; transient contrast changes; Blake, Westendorf, & Fox,
1990; Walker & Powell,
1979; Wilson, Blake, & Lee,
2001; or a delay in the onset of the image of one eye; Wolfe,
1984) can determine percept dominance. Our findings indicate that saccades, due to their resulting retinal image changes, can truly cause perceptual alternations in a similar manner as the abovementioned changes in the stimulus.
The occurrence of binocular rivalry with retinal afterimages has frequently led to the assumption that the role of eye movements in binocular rivalry is negligible. Here, we have shown that for real images, this assumption is not valid, but, even for afterimages, a role of eye movements cannot be ruled out because the perception of afterimages is known to depend on eye movements. For instance, afterimages tend to disappear (e.g., Kennard, Hartmann, Kraft, & Boshes,
1970) and reappear after the execution of a saccade (e.g., Ditchburn,
1973; McDougall,
1903). For stabilized image rivalry (by controlling for occurring eye movements), the stabilization of the image on the retina is usually not perfect, otherwise the images would perceptually disappear (Ditchburn & Ginsborg,
1952; Martinez-Conde et al.,
2006; Riggs, Ratliff, Cornsweet, & Cornsweet,
1953), which means that the influence of eye movements during stabilized image rivalry remains to be verified.
Perceptual alternations can occur without saccades and without stimulus changes (return to chance level for the perceived grating orientation in our
Experiment 3), indicating a central process. Covert spatial attention is such a central process through which percept dominance can be influenced (Brouwer, Tong, Schwarzbach, & van Ee,
2005; Slotnick & Yantis,
2005; von Helmholtz,
1910). Our finding that retinal image changes due to microsaccades can trigger perceptual alternations must be taken into account when studying the role of attention. In addition, several studies demonstrated that spatial attention shifts are usually accompanied by microsaccades in the direction of the attention shift even under strict fixation tasks (Engbert & Kliegl,
2003; Hafed & Clark,
2002; Laubrock, Engbert, & Kliegl,
2005). Thus, a correlation between attention shifts and perceptual alternations might potentially be due to the retinal image changes resulting from co-occurring microsaccades rather than the attention shifts per se.
Because only a restricted class of eye movements (those that change the foveal image) can cause a change in awareness, the preparation of an eye movement is not sufficient to cause a change in awareness (unless the saccade landing position would be accurately known, which presumably is not the case given the large errors of saccades to well-defined targets; van Opstal & van Gisbergen,
1989). The “premotor theory” (Rizzolatti, Riggio, Dascola, & Umiltá,
1987) posits that the allocation of covert spatial attention is equivalent to planning but not executing a saccade. According to this theory, our results suggest that covert spatial attention shifts (top–down influence) cannot be responsible for perceptual alternations in binocular rivalry or at least that this top–down influence is much weaker than the bottom–up influence through the retinal image changes. Indeed, for binocular rivalry, there is evidence that voluntary control has only little influence on percept dominance (Meng & Tong,
2004; van Ee, van Dam, & Brouwer,
2005) unless the bottom–up stream of information is changed either continuously (Chong, Tadin, & Blake,
2005) or abruptly (Chong & Blake,
2006; Mitchell, Stoner, & Reynolds,
2004).