These findings are consistent with the more general claim that attentional processing is not limited to a fixed number of items (Davis,
2004; Davis, Welch, Holmes, & Shepherd,
2001; Tripathy & Barret,
2004; Tripathy, Narasimhan, & Barret,
2007). For example, the ability to discriminate changes in the trajectory of moving items drops off dramatically as the number of tracked trajectories increases beyond 1 (Tripathy & Barret,
2004). This suggests that the resolution required to detect a deviation is the primary limit on the number of trajectories that can be tracked, not the number of trajectories (Tripathy et al.,
2007). While our conclusions are similar, there are several reasons to believe that the constraints on trajectory tracking are different than those on multiple object tracking. First, the trajectory tracking task places heavy demands on visual memory. To determine whether an item has changed direction, it is necessary to compare its current direction to its previous direction. Indeed, visual memory limitations may be the primary determinant of the limits on trajectory tracking (Narasimhan, Tripathy, & Barret,
2005). In contrast, the multiple-object tracking task does not require a direct comparison of the current features of an object to its previous features, and iconic memory is unlikely to play an essential role in this task. Second, observers with amblyopia are impaired in multiple object tracking (Ho et al.,
2006), but not in the trajectory task (Levi & Tripathy,
2006). Thus, while both multiple object tracking and trajectory tracking appear to be resource-limited, the resource in multiple object tracking appears to be attention (consistent with the attentional resolution results of
Experiment 2), whereas in trajectory tracking it appears to be visual sensory memory.