Our own results indicate that second-order vision across the visual field is inherently organized on the basis of the ratio between carrier and envelope spatial frequencies. In simple terms, it cares fundamentally about the relationship between its own spatial scale and that of the first-order input which it receives, and subsequent analysis of the output of second-order filters is arranged on the basis of this relationship. Thus, while second-order vision consists of units with a wide range of preferred
f carr/
f env ratios at each eccentricity (and sensitivity is determined by this ratio), all share a common eccentricity dependence. The observation of carrier frequency selectivity in second-order vision is compatible with both neurophysiological (Baker,
1999; Baker & Mareschal,
2001; Mareschal & Baker,
1998; Zhou & Baker,
1994) and psychophysical findings (McGraw et al.,
1999). While the lack of significant crossover adaptation between first- and second-order vision is now well established (Nishida et al.,
1997; Schofield & Georgeson,
1999), McGraw et al. (
1999) investigated crossover adaptation within the second-order pathway for stimuli of different carrier spatial frequencies and orientations. They demonstrated that adaptation to second-order stimuli transferred across carrier orientation but not across carrier spatial frequency. In other words, second-order stimuli of sufficiently different
fcarr/
fenv ratios do not interact. Thus, while carrier orientation information is lost to subsequent analysis of second-order vision through a process of pooling (Arsenault, Wilkinson, & Kingdom,
1999; McGraw et al.,
1999), information regarding relative spatial scale is strictly retained. As further evidence for the importance of relative spatial scale, it should be noted that illusory interactions between first- and second-order vision, such as the Fraser and Zöllner illusions of orientation, are critically dependent upon the
fcarr/
fenv ratio of the stimuli used (Skillen, Whitaker, Popple, & McGraw,
2002). If this ratio is held constant, the magnitude of the illusory effects is also constant, demonstrating the phenomenon of ‘scale invariance’ (Jamar & Koenderink,
1985; Kingdom & Keeble,
1999; Sutter et al.,
1995). Scale invariant analysis has distinct real-world relevance, where objects are continually changing in size either through their own motion or self-motion of the observer. Either way, while the spatial scale of the object's first-order texture and its second-order variation in texture might change dramatically, the ratio between the two is maintained, thereby permitting continued analysis of the object within the same
fcarr/
fenv processing stream.