The mask in
Experiment 2 was chosen to maximize the effect of surround suppression. It had a different shape and a larger spatial extent than mask in
Experiment 1. The larger spatial extent as well as the mask's distinct shape could permit some figure/background mechanisms to operate between the mask and the target. It is conceivable that such mechanisms could make the surround suppression more symmetric. Also, in the crowding experiment, the task was orientation discrimination, whereas in the surround-suppression experiment, the task was contrast detection. Is the loss of the inward–outward asymmetry in
Experiment 2 simply due to the different tasks and/or the different spatial configurations of the stimuli?
In an attempt to answer this question, we first modified
Experiment 2 as follows. Both the stimulus and the task in the control experiment were identical to
Experiment 1, except that the spatial frequency of the stimulus was fixed at 1.5 cpd (the same as in
Experiment 2), whereas its contrast varied to determine the orientation identification threshold. The 1.5-cpd stimuli were used to keep the target–mask separation (4
λ = 2.7°) large enough to avoid any significant crowding. Recall that in
Experiment 1 the sizes of the target, plaid mask, and their separation were reduced together until the subject could no longer identify the orientation of the target.
Two subjects (SPM and JF) carried out the control experiment. No significant surround suppression was observed. This is in a stark contrast with strong crowding observed in
Experiment 1 with the same mask. This also agrees with our preliminary studies, which showed that when the surround mask is of the same size as the target very little or no suppression is produced, even when the mask is positioned immediately next to the target. Note that even with the half-annulus mask, the effect of surround suppression in
Experiment 2 was smaller than the effect of (inward) crowding in
Experiment 1. Our results support Levi et al. (
2002) in that surround suppression is weaker than crowding.
To determine what spatial scale induces the onset of crowding, SPM repeated the control experiment for 3 and 5 cpd stimuli. There was very little or no masking effect at 3 cpd, but at 5 cpd a significant crowding was observed. The effect was very similar to the crowding observed in
Experiment 1, including strong inward–outward anisotropy. Of course, this was to be expected, given that at 5 cpd the control experiment was almost identical to
Experiment 1. Thus, unlike surround suppression, crowding only occurs when the Gabor target is near the acuity limit; 5 cpd is about an octave larger than the resolution limit at 9° eccentricity.
Because the first control did not produce enough suppression, we modified
Experiment 2 in a different way. The stimulus duration was reduced from 150 ms to ∼36 ms (3 video frames at 85 Hz screen refresh rate). Because surround suppression increases dramatically at short durations (Petrov, Carandini, & McKee,
2006), we expected that a small mask would produce strong suppression in this case. The mask was a single Gabor patch shown at 45% contrast. Otherwise it matched the target Gabor in slant (45° to the right), spatial frequency (3 cpd), and phase (cos). The slant and the increased spatial frequency were chosen to match stimuli in
Experiment 1 more closely (see
Figure 1a). The separation between the mask and the target was 4
λ, the same as in
Experiment 1. Only the inward and outward mask positions were tested.
Two observers (YP and SPM) carried out the control experiment. The results shown by star symbols in
Figure 3 demonstrate that the Gabor mask produced equally strong suppression at inward and outward locations. Thus, neither the extent of the mask nor its shape explains the no-effect of the mask location in
Experiment 2.
Altogether, the results of the first two experiments and the control experiments provide a clear demonstration that crowding and surround suppression are distinct phenomena. The small plaid mask produced strong crowding but no surround suppression. The crowding was characterized by the strong inward–outward anisotropy, whereas surround suppression was isotropic for both small and large surrounds. The inward–outward anisotropy in the crowding experiment is in agreement with Bouma (
1973) and later studies which also showed that the inward letter of a word is much harder to identify than the outward letter. Thus, the anisotropy is not an artifact of the stimulus we used here but a general (and rather intriguing) property of crowding. Our results also argue against defining crowding as a “letter only” phenomenon, as suggested by Pelli et al. (
2004).
We note that this anisotropy is too large to be accounted for by cortical magnification. The outward mask, at threshold, was on the average 1° more peripheral than the inward mask in
Experiment 1. Given that the whole stimulus was at 9° eccentricity, the 11% difference in the mask eccentricity is insufficient to explain the fivefold increase in crowding based on cortical topography alone. Besides, Petrov and McKee (
2006) noted that the stronger radial effects cannot be explained by cortical magnification as data from primate and human retinotopy (Adams & Horton,
2003; Schira, Kontsevich, & Tyler,
2005; Tootell, Switkes, Silverman, & Hamiltonet,
1988; Van Essen, Newsome, & Maunsell,
1984) suggest that a radial–tangential anisotropy determined by equal cortical distance would, if anything, be in the opposite direction. This is because, at least along the vertical meridian, in order to travel an equal visual distance radially and tangentially, one must travel farther on cortex in the radial direction due to overlap when crossing ocular dominance columns. Along the horizontal meridian, the arrangement of columns is more haphazard and there is no particular cortical anisotropy other than magnification. In summary, these crowding anisotropies are not easily explained by retinotopy or cortical magnification, at least in primary visual cortex.
The anisotropy clearly distinguishes crowding from surround suppression. We will next use this anisotropy as a litmus test to test whether a commonly used “crowding” paradigm is measuring crowding or surround suppression. In this paradigm, the experimenter determines the contrast required to identify a target in the presence of flanking targets. The contrast of the flanks is kept constant, and it is normally considerably higher than the contrast of the test target. We note that this contrast threshold paradigm has been extensively used in recent years. For example, most of the important crowding studies cited in
Table 1 (Chung, Levi, & Legge,
2001; Levi et al.,
2002; Pelli et al.,
2004; Strasburger et al.,
1991) used this paradigm.