Recently, there has been a debate over the origin of aftereffects following crowded adaptation (Blake, Tadin, Sobel, Raissian, & Chong,
2006; He, Cavanagh, & Intriligator,
1996; Intriligator & Cavanagh,
2001). Our results suggest that following crowded adaptation (of any stimulus), the measured local aftereffect could be the result of both local adaptation and global (or remote) adaptation from surrounding patterns. This is important because it could cause an underestimation (or overestimation) of the magnitude of the local aftereffect in previous studies (Aghdaee,
2005; Aghdaee & Zandvakili,
2005; Blake et al.,
2006; He et al.,
1996; He, Cavanagh, & Intriligator,
1997; Montaser-Kouhsari & Rajimehr,
2005; Rajimehr,
2004a,
2004b; Rajimehr, Montaser-Kouhsari, & Afraz,
2003; Rajimehr, Vaziri-Pashkam, Afraz, & Esteky,
2004; Whitney,
2005,
2006). In motion crowding studies, for example, the random directions of motion in the crowder stimuli usually average to zero but could serve as a global dynamic noise pattern, effectively raising thresholds across all directions and reducing the measured aftereffect. The same logic (or other types of global adaptation effects) could hold for other features, such as orientation or form. Further, the extent of spatial pooling or global adaptation might be contrast dependent or even stimulus specific (e.g., only within the second-order motion pathway in these experiments). For example, for first-order stimuli, the extent of spatial pooling increases with decreasing stimulus contrast (Sceniak, Ringach, Hawken, & Shapley,
1999; Tadin, Lappin, Gilroy, & Blake,
2003). Global first-order adaptation effects might therefore manifest themselves disproportionately at lower stimulus contrasts.