Motion information is a central part of our visual experience. This is reflected in observers' high sensitivity to motion; under ideal circumstances, we can reliably discriminate motion stimuli that are displaced by only a few arc seconds (Nakayama & Silverman,
1985) or presented for a few milliseconds (Lappin, Tadin, Nyquist, & Corn,
2009). However, motion information is used for purposes other than motion perception per se (Nakayama,
1985). It can contribute to a wide range of brain functions, including the generation of tracking eye movements, altering hand movements, controlling posture, aiding in figure-ground segmentation, and calculating three-dimensional structure (Born, Groh, Zhao, & Lukasewycz,
2000; Bradley, Chang, & Andersen,
1998; Lisberger, Morris, & Tychsen,
1987; Nakayama,
1985; Saijo, Murakami, Nishida, & Gomi,
2005). These brain functions have widely varying requirements and goals, suggesting that associated neural mechanisms likely have varying sensitivities that are matched to specific demands of different motion processes (Gomi, Abekawa, & Shimojo,
2013). Therefore, a stimulus that generates a weak or unreliable perceptual sensation of motion may nonetheless be quite suitable for another brain function.