Optic flow provides the observer with useful information such as direction of heading (Gibson,
1950; Lappe, Bremmer, & van den Berg,
1999; Warren & Hannon,
1988), time to contact (Lee,
1980), distance traveled (Redlick, Jenkin, & Harris,
2001), segmentation of object motion (Logan & Duffy,
2006; Warren & Rushton,
2008), and the slant and tilt (pose) of surfaces in the scene (Koenderink,
1986). And there is good evidence that the human visual system takes advantage of some of these. For example, observers can identify the focus of expansion (Bex & Falkenberg,
2006; Warren, Morris, & Kalish,
1988) and make accurate judgements of their heading based on this (Crowell & Banks,
1993), even in the presence of head and eye movements (Royden, Banks, & Crowell,
1992; Royden & Picone,
2007). A large-scale (global) analysis of the expansion component in optic flow is used to judge time-to-contact (Giachritsis & Harris,
2005), rotation is used to compensate retinal flow for observer roll (Hanada & Ejima,
2000), expansion and rotation influence posture (Lee & Aronson,
1974; Richards, Mulavara, & Bloomberg,
2004), and deforming random-dot displays give a profound sense of moving surfaces with distinct slant and tilt (Domini & Caudek,
1999; Freeman, Harris, & Meese,
1996; Meese & Harris,
1997; Meese, Harris, & Freeman,
1995; Zhong, Cornilleau-Pérès, Cheong, Yeow, & Droulez,
2006). Thus, there is a clear functional distinction between spiral space, which provides information about the observer's position within, and relation to the environment (ego-motion), and deformation space, which provides information about the environment's layout and structure.