Existing mechanistic theories of illusory contour processing can be divided into three broad classes: contour interpolation (Dresp & Grossberg,
1997; Grossberg & Mingolla,
1985; Grossberg, Mingolla, & Ross,
1997; Grossberg & Yazdanbakhsh,
2005; Guttman & Kellman,
2004; Kellman & Shipley,
1991; Raizada & Grossberg,
2003; Singh & Hoffman,
1999), contour extrapolation (Field, Hayes, & Hess,
1993; Li,
1998; Pillow & Rubin,
2002; Shipley & Kellman,
2003; Singh & Fulvio,
2005), and figural feedback (Albert & Hoffman,
2000; Anderson,
1997; Gregory,
1972; Grossberg et al.,
1997; Lee,
2002; Nakayama & Shimojo,
1990,
1992; Raizada & Grossberg,
2003; Rock & Anson,
1979). Contour interpolation mechanisms inwardly connect pairs of aligned inducer fragments to close contours across intervening gaps, for example a bipole uniting two edges in the Boundary Contour System (Grossberg & Mingolla,
1985). Contour extrapolation mechanisms extend outward from a single fragment, for example by taking a random walk outward from inducers (Williams & Jacobs,
1997). In contrast to these contour-based computations, figural feedback mechanisms suggest that the contour fragments support the perception of a surface or figure and that this figural representation feeds back to fill in contour gaps, for example by combining visual cues to a surface (Carman & Welch,
1992). Contour interpolation and contour extrapolation are often observed neurophysiologically and frequently described as either feed-forward or local cooperative processing mechanisms (Ffytche & Zeki,
1996; Grosof, Shapley, & Hawken,
1993; Hirsch et al.,
1995; Lee & Nguyen,
2001; Ramsden, Hung, & Roe,
2001; Seghier et al.,
2000; Sheth, Sharma, Rao, & Sur,
1996; von der Heydt, Peterhans, & Baumgartner,
1984), and evidence for figural feedback is often observed in later visual areas (Mendola, Dale, Fischl, Liu, & Tootell,
1999; Stanley & Rubin,
2003).