First, a central question that has plagued attention research is how such differing results could be obtained in electrophysiological and brain imaging studies of spatial attention in early visual cortex (e.g., reviewed in Heeger & Ress,
2002; Pessoa, Kastner, & Ungerleider,
2003; Posner & Gilbert,
1999). fMRI experiments routinely demonstrate large signal increases whereas electrophysiology studies rarely show increases in spike rate. The differences in the results have been particularly striking in V1 where some electrophysiology has shown either no (Luck et al.,
1997; Marcus & Van Essen,
2002) or very small (e.g., 6%, McAdams & Maunsell,
1999) increases with attention. However, see Ito and Gilbert (
1999), Motter (
1993), Roberts, Delicato, Herrero, Gieselmann, and Thiele (
2007), and Roelfsema et al. (
1998) for studies demonstrating considerably larger stimulus-evoked changes with attention in early visual areas. Possible explanations for these apparently conflicting results have ranged from the use of different species (monkey vs. human) to intrinsic differences in the measurements. For example, because the fMRI signal is tied to hemodynamics, it has been suggested that subthreshold or inhibitory processes—which could contribute to blood flow responses but would be less apparent using electrophysiological techniques—are contributing to the fMRI results (Heeger & Ress,
2002). Also, others have suggested that V1 attention effects occur late, as the possible result of feedback from extrastriate areas (Martínez et al.,
1999; Noesselt et al.,
2002). Since fMRI is integrating the response over long time scales, perhaps the signal incorporates both initial feedforward and longer-latency feedback influences from other areas. Although this may be true, the additional presumption of this explanation is that previous electrophysiology studies have simply missed the hypothesized later occurring effects of attention in V1.