Of course, our results should note be taken as evidence that grouping is necessarily preattentive. There is behavioral evidence to suggest that our perception of grouping and figure-ground relies on a sophisticated interplay of multiple visual processes located throughout the processing stream (Beck & Palmer,
2002; Ben-Av, Sagi, & Braun,
1992; Mack, Tang, Tuma, Kahn, & Rock,
1992; Palmer, Neff, & Beck,
1996; Palmer & Nelson,
2000; Peterson, Harvey, & Weidenbacher,
1991; Rock & Brosgole,
1964; Rock, Nijhawan, Palmer, & Tudor,
1992), and thus perceptual organization is best thought of as a result of both feedforward and feedback mechanisms. However, we should point out that, regardless of whether grouping involves feedback or recurrent connections, these processes have their origin in the parameters of the visual stimulus, and thus grouping can still be considered primarily stimulus driven. Moreover, the effects found here should be contrasted sharply with the top-down effects of directed attention (Kastner et al.,
1998; Luck et al.,
1997; Moran & Desimone,
1985; Motter,
1993; Recanzone & Wurtz,
2000; Reynolds et al.,
1999). In the directed attention studies, the subjects were required to “select” one of several competing targets. The result was a reduction in suppressive effects. In the case of the homogeneous stimuli, selection is not necessary. Indeed, it is more difficult to select one of a group of similar items (Driver & Baylis,
1989; Duncan & Humpreys,
1989; Kramer & Jacobson,
1991) than one of a set of heterogeneous items. Instead, the reduced competition found in homogeneous displays is more likely due to aggregative mechanisms rather than segregative ones.