There are, however, other mechanisms that may flatten the 1/
f temporal amplitude spectrum. According to channel theory, masking is caused by a reduction in the signal-to-noise ratio associated with a signal-relevant channel's response. In masking, signal-to-noise ratios may be reduced by either reducing the signal (via an active inhibitory process; Cass & Alais,
2006; Graham, Chandler, & Field,
2006; Srinivasan, Laughlin, & Dubs,
1982; or an inactive one, e.g.,
synaptic depression; Chance, Nelson, & Abbott,
1998) and/or increasing the noise. Temporal whitening could therefore, in principle, be due to the low temporal frequency channel being more susceptible to high temporal frequency-driven noise than the converse situation (see
Figure 6b). One can conceive of several processing architectures capable of producing such an asymmetric noisy interaction. The output of high temporal frequency-biased mechanisms may, for example, disrupt the efficiency that low temporal frequency information is encoded and/or transmitted. This may be accomplished by noisy interactions between otherwise parallel pathways (e.g., transient and sustained channels) or else temporal mechanisms with differential susceptibilities to input noise from a common source (Chance et al.,
1998; Langley & Bex,
2007; Wang, Liu, Sanchez-Vives, & McCormick,
2003). We refer to this general class of models as the
asymmetric noise hypothesis.
Equation 3: asymmetric inhibition and asymmetric noise model of masking, where
a1&2 = amplitude and
b1&2 = bandwidth of Gaussian peaks (low and high temporal frequency) derived from masking of 1-Hz target (red curve in
Figure 6a);
a3 = amplitude and
b3 = bandwidth of Gaussian peak derived from masking of 15-Hz target (blue curve in
Figure 6a). Note that the subtractive term between fist and second Gaussian components results from an active inhibitory process in the asymmetric inhibition model and from input noise in the asymmetric noise model. See
Figure 6b for graphical representation.