Therefore, we introduced (1) a novel action of point-light stimulus and (2) an adaptation paradigm to investigate the function of each component. Adaptation is the phenomenon in which the firing rates of responsive neurons and blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signals are reduced when a particular stimulus is presented repetitively (Grill-Spector, Henson, & Martin,
2006; Krekelberg, Boynton, & van Wezel,
2006). The advantage of introducing an adaptation paradigm is that it enables the functional properties of cortical neurons to be probed in two stages, (adaptation and test phases) (Grill-Spector & Malach,
2001): if the signal remains adapted in the test phase despite a change of stimulus, it will indicate that the neurons are invariant to that attribute; if, however, the signal recovers in the test phase from the adapted state, it would imply that the neurons are sensitive to the property that was altered. Recent studies have revealed an adaptation effect not only for basic low-level visual dimensions, such as motion, orientation, spatial frequency or texture (Anstis, Verstraten, & Mather,
1998; Clifford,
2002; Durgin & Proffitt,
1996), but also for high-level visual processing, such as that of faces (Leopold, O'Toole, Vetter, & Blanz,
2001; Webster, Kaping, Mizokami, & Duhamel,
2004) or body parts (Kovács et al.,
2006). This adaptation paradigm has also intensively introduced into ERP studies of motion perception (Heinrich, van der Smagt, Bach, & Hoffmann,
2004; Hoffmann, Unsöld, & Bach,
2001). It has been demonstrated that a negative deflection with a latency of around 150–200 ms (the N2 component in these studies) reflects cortical motion processing in humans (Bach & Ullrich,
1994; Heinrich et al.,
2004; Hoffmann, Dorn, & Bach,
1999; Kubová, Kuba, Spekreijse, & Blakemore,
1995; Probst, Plendl, Paulus, Wist, & Scherg,
1993) and that this component is very susceptible to motion adaptation (Bach & Ullrich,
1994; Göpfert, Müller, & Hartwig,
1984; Wist, Gross, & Niedeggen,
1994). These studies demonstrated that the amplitude of the N2 component was reduced during the test phase when the motion direction of the test stimulus is identical to that of the adaptation stimulus (Hoffmann et al.,
2001) or when both motion direction and velocity of the test stimulus are identical to those of the adaptation stimulus (Heinrich et al.,
2004). These findings suggest that we should be able to specify the attributes of the visual stimulus that modulates the ERP component by introducing an adaptation paradigm. We presumed that if the observed components (N1 and N2) were functionally distinct, then each component would be modulated by a specific adaptation stimulus. Alternatively, if the components in the test phase functionally overlapped, the components would be equally modulated by the adaptation stimulus. In this study, we used an adaptation paradigm to reveal how both local and global motion information modulates each component. We used thee adaptation stimuli: point-light motion (PLM), scrambled PLM (sPLM) and a static point-light stimulus. For the sPLM stimulus, the number of point-lights and the velocity vectors of point-lights were identical to those of the PLM stimulus, but the initial starting positions were randomized. For the static point-light stimulus, the initial frame of the adaptation stimuli was presented during the adaptation phase. In the test phase, we used identical PLM and sPLM stimuli to those in the adaptation phase (for precise experimental settings, see below) and investigated how local and global motion information affects the neural responses to the test stimuli.