Although the consequences of microsaccades, e.g., for perception and fixation stability, have been studied extensively (see Martinez-Conde, Macknick, & Hubel,
2004, for a review), little is known about their implementation in the oculomotor system. It is clear, however, that despite their different amplitudes and traditional subdivision into different categories of eye movements, microsaccades and large-scale saccades share a wide range of characteristics. First, both are typically binocular eye movements with almost identical amplitudes and directions in both eyes (Ditchburn & Ginsborg,
1953; Krauskopf, Cornsweet, & Riggs,
1960; Lord,
1951). Second, both fall on the main sequence (Zuber, Stark, & Cook,
1965), i.e., the relationship between peak velocity and amplitude in these movements follows a linear relationship. Third, both result in a strong elevation of the visual perceptual threshold covering some time around the movement (saccadic suppression; Beeler,
1967; Latour,
1962; Volkmann,
1962; Volkmann, Schick, & Riggs,
1968; Zuber, Crider, & Stark,
1964; Zuber & Stark,
1966). Fourth, it was argued that inter-saccadic intervals in reading have similar distributions as inter-microsaccadic intervals during simple fixation of a letter (Cunitz & Steinman,
1969). Fifth, microsaccades are subject to voluntary control. Their rate can be reduced intentionally (Fiorentini & Ercoles,
1966; Steinman, Cunitz, Timberlake, & Herman,
1967) and very few are produced in high-acuity observational (Bridgeman & Palca,
1980) and finely guided visuomotor tasks (Winterson & Collewijn,
1976). Some subjects may even generate saccades as small as microsaccades voluntarily (Haddad & Steinman,
1973). Sixth, there is a strong relationship between spatial attention and the generation of saccades. Saccades are virtually always preceded by shifts of covert attention (e.g., Deubel & Schneider,
1996; Kowler, Anderson, Dosher, & Blaser,
1995) and both covert attention and saccades have strongly overlapping neurophysiological foundations (e.g., Corbetta et al.,
1998; Kustov & Robinson,
1996). A pronounced correlation was also found for covert attention and microsaccades (Engbert & Kliegl,
2003b; Galfano, Betta, & Turatto,
2004; Hafed & Clark,
2002; Laubrock, Engbert, & Kliegl,
2005; Laubrock, Engbert, Rolfs, & Kliegl,
2007; Rolfs, Engbert, & Kliegl,
2004,
2005).