At the core of the model being proposed lie neurons sensitive to specific pictorial subregions or broad shape cues such as head shape. Some features may correspond to simple nameable parts, but it seems likely that this would be the exception rather than the rule. The best source of information which we currently have as to what these features might be like comes from single cell recording and optical imaging studies in the macaque. This work has revealed cells responsive selectively to faces (or other objects), which can be effectively stimulated by subparts of a full face (or object) stimulus (Logothetis,
2000; Tanaka et al.,
1991; Tsunoda et al.,
2001; Wang, Tanaka, & Tanifuji,
1996; Yamane et al.,
1988), consistent with a piecewise, feature-based mode of representation. However, attempts made with other neurons have often failed to produce an effective substimulus, leading some researchers to suggest that holistic representations are present too (Desimone,
1991; Logothetis,
2000; Tanaka et al.,
1991). In practice, finding an effective substimulus may be an almost impossible task under normal, time limited, cellular recording conditions, especially for highly specialized neurons. A more feasible route to establishing the level of stimulus specificity is to test an array of same-category objects (e.g., other faces, in the case of face cells). Studies of this type on face cells suggest that even neurons from the most anterior parts of the temporal lobe respond to many of the faces tested (Abbott, Rolls, & Tovee,
1996; Perrett, Hietanen, Oram, & Benson,
1992; Young & Yamane,
1992). What is more, it seems unlikely that representations become any more specialized beyond this level since similar levels of stimulus selectivity have been reported for neurons in areas receiving input from the temporal lobe, such as the ventral striatum, frontal lobe, and amygdala (Leonard, Rolls, Wilson, & Baylis,
1985; Williams, Rolls, Leonard, & Stern,
1993; Wilson, Scalaidhe, & Goldman-Rakic,
1993).