In the behavioral repertoire of an organism, most actions depend at least to some degree on an internal sense of elapsed time. From sleep–wake cycles and foraging behavior to pursuit-evasion and fine motor control, successful execution depends on the ability to perceive and reproduce different intervals of time (Buhusi & Meck,
2005). Time as an explicit phenomenon has been studied for years in the psychology literature, in both humans and animals (Gallistel & Gibbon,
2000; Mauk & Buonomano,
2004). In the class of experiments referred to as “interval reproduction,” subjects are typically presented a standard reference time interval, delineated by tones or visual cues. They are then required to reproduce the reference interval using a manual action such as a button press. The subjects match the reference with a precision that depends on the stimulus length (Gibbon, Malapani, Dale, & Gallistel,
1997), the cue modality (Penney,
2003), and other factors (Merchant, Zarco, & Prado,
2008). A simultaneous task, such as monitoring additional intervals (Brown, Stubbs, & West,
1992; Brown & West,
1990), causes the subject to overestimate durations and therefore respond later. This effect could be due to the allocation of attention to a concurrent process that could change the perception of elapsed time (Casini & Macar,
1997; Fortin,
2003; Macar, Grondin, & Casini,
1994).