Results (
Figure 2) showed clear evidence for a ‘dipper.’ JND's were comparatively high when one of the patterns had no variance (the leftmost point on the graphs) and fell as variance was added. The curves in
Figure 2 show the best fits to the data. (Note that these are not fits to the data points in the graph but are rather maximum likelihood fits (found with the FMINSEARCH function of MATLAB) of the model to data vectors consisting of the pedestal level, added signal level, and observer's response on every trial of the experiment.) These best-fitting parameter values and their associated log likelihoods are shown in
Table 1.
A likelihood ratio test was used to compare the fits of the two models, one with and one without a threshold. Let
L C and
L U be the likelihoods of the best-fitting constrained and unconstrained models. As is well-known (e.g., Hoel, Port, & Stone,
1971), under the null hypothesis that the constrained model captures the true state of the world,
is asymptotically distributed as chi-square with 1 degree of freedom (for the single additional free parameter in
LU). The chi-square values were significant (
p < .010) for observers MM and IM, but not for observer JAS. To give a more intuitive impression of the success of the two models,
Figure 3 plots the relative likelihoods in comparison to two extreme baselines. The ‘coin flipping’ model has the simulated observer choose between the two intervals with equal probability, independently of the stimulus level or pedestal. This is as poor as a fit could be. The ‘Weibull fits’ model shows the best fit of a set of 2-parameter Weibull psychometric functions to the each of the pedestal conditions separately. This model has 2
n free parameters, where
n is the number of pedestals, in comparison to the 2 and 3 parameters of the models described in
Table 1, and it is as good as a fit could be given the noise in the observer's data. It is satisfying to see that the models are much closer to the Weibull fits than to ‘coin flipping’. The two versions of the intrinsic noise model, with and without an additional threshold, are seen to be very close.
Finally, to see if the threshold nonlinearity giving rise to the dipper was modifiable by experience, one observer (MM) undertook an extensive series of observations with a zero pedestal to see if performance would improve. Results (shown in
Figure 4) failed to find any evidence for learning.