Importantly, CDA magnitude is also modulated by the type of features currently maintained in VWM (Gao et al.,
2009; Woodman & Vogel,
2008) and not by the number of spatial locations (Gao et al.,
2011; Ikkai, McCollough, & Vogel,
2010). For example, remembering the orientation of a conjunction object results in larger CDA than remembering the color (Woodman & Vogel,
2008), and presenting objects sequentially at the same or different locations results in comparably sized CDA amplitudes (Ikkai et al.,
2010). Although it has been questioned whether CDA codes the number of maintained
objects rather than the number of
features (Luria, Sessa, Gotler, Jolicoeur, & Dell'Acqua,
2010; Luria & Vogel,
2011a), there is consensus in the literature that CDA is not a neural correlate of spatial working memory (Gao et al.,
2011; Ikkai et al.,
2010).
1 As well, CDA appears to reflect the actual information content of VWM rather than the perceptual difficulty associated with encoding that information (Ikkai et al.,
2010), and CDA magnitude can be modulated, moment by moment, by the number of lateralized targets in a multiple-object tracking task (Drew, Horowitz, Wolfe, & Vogel,
2012; Drew & Vogel,
2008). This demonstration of CDA modulation in a tracking task is particularly interesting in the current context, as it suggests that CDA may index demands on both VWM and attention (e.g., Drew et al.,
2012), although it is also possible that attended objects may be temporarily stored in VWM (Emrich, Al-Aidroos, Pratt, & Ferber,
2009,
2010). Taken together, these studies suggest that CDA amplitude provides a reliable estimate of VWM load and is therefore a useful tool to measure how the VWM load imposed by a target representation affects later search performance.