Although we observed spatial-frequency tuning characteristics for all print sizes, the tuning functions are not identical.
Figures 4 and
5 show that the peak tuning frequency progressively shifts toward higher object spatial frequency, in units of c/letter, when print size increases at the fovea (repeated measures ANOVA:
F (df=4,12) = 60.25, Greenhouse–Geisser adjusted
p = 0.0002) and 10° eccentricity (repeated measures ANOVA:
F (df=2,6) = 76.65, Greenhouse–Geisser adjusted
p = 0.0009). Averaged across the four observers, the peak tuning frequency shifted from 1.7 to 3.4 c/letter when print size increased from 1.2 to 16× CPS at the fovea, and 3.4 to 4.9 c/letter when print size increased from 1.2 to 4× CPS at 10° inferior visual field. This shift in peak tuning frequency with print size is reminiscent of the finding for letter identification, in which the peak tuning frequency also demonstrates a progressive shift toward higher object spatial frequency with increased letter size, for single or crowded letters (Chung, Legge et al.,
2002; Chung, Levi et al.,
2002; Chung & Tjan,
2007). When the peak tuning frequency is converted to retinal frequency in c/deg and plotted as a function of the reciprocal of letter size, the exponent of the resulting spatial-frequency scaling function is approximately 0.6–0.7, indicating only partial scale dependence. Here, to quantify the scale dependency of reading, we plot the spatial-frequency scaling function for reading in
Figure 6. Data are pooled across all observers and are plotted separately for the fovea (
Figure 6A) and 10° eccentricity (
Figure 6B). A power function fit to each data set yielded an exponent of 0.70 ± 0.02 at the fovea and 0.60 ± 0.05 at 10° eccentricity. These values are very similar to those obtained for identification of single or crowded letters (Chung, Legge et al.,
2002; Chung, Levi et al.,
2002; Chung & Tjan,
2007; Majaj et al.,
2002). However, even when the exponents are comparable between the reading speed and letter identification data, the two sets of data can still be offset vertically. Therefore, in
Figure 6, we also include the data for single letter identification (small gray symbols), replotted from Chung, Legge et al. (
2002). Clearly, the data for single letter identification closely match those for reading speed at the fovea. At 10° eccentricity, there seems to be a shift in spatial scale between the reading speed and the letter identification data. The significance of the good match of the data between reading speed and single letter identification at the fovea and the demonstrated shift in spatial scale at 10° eccentricity will be addressed in the
Discussion section.