A gradual decline in visual abilities is common even in healthy aging. These changes may be explained, in part, by optical characteristics of the aging eye (Weale,
1963) but are attributed mainly to neural changes in the aging brain (Ball & Sekuler,
1986; Habak & Faubert,
2000; Sekuler & Sekuler,
2000; Spear,
1993). For example, neuronal activity in aging macaque V1 displays an increase in spontaneous firing, a decrease in signal-to-noise ratio, and a decrease of selectivity for both orientation and motion direction (Leventhal, Wang, Pu, Zhou, & Ma,
2003; Schmolesky, Wang, Pu, & Leventhal,
2000), all of which are exacerbated in V2 (Yu, Wang, Li, Zhou, & Leventhal,
2006). Basic sensitivities mediated by early stages of visual processing, such as contrast and spatial frequency, deteriorate with age (Owsley, Sekuler, & Siemsen,
1983). Aging also affects complex representations such as face perception (Habak, Wilkinson, & Wilson,
2008; Lott, Haegerstrom-Portnoy, Schneck, & Brabyn,
2005; Owsley, Sekuler, & Boldt,
1981), which rely upon higher stages of the object-processing stream. Surprisingly, thresholds for detecting deformation of circular shapes, which are hyperacuities in young adults (Wilkinson, Wilson, & Habak,
1998), appear to be preserved into advanced age, at least for low radial frequencies (Wang,
2001; Wang, Wilson, Locke, & Edwards,
2002). The global pooling of contour information underlying this task is thought to occur at intermediate levels of the ventral pathway (Gallant, Shoup, & Mazer,
2000; Wilkinson et al.,
2000), yet the optical and neural degradation of the input associated with aging do not appear to impair this ability. However, Wang et al.'s studies used only luminance-defined radial frequency (RF) contours, and stimulus exposure time was relatively long (500 ms minimum).