When multiple objects are presented in proximity to a peripheral target, the identification of that target can be strongly impaired, a phenomenon that is referred to as “crowding” or “lateral masking” (Bouma,
1970; Huckauf & Heller,
2004; Pelli, Palomares, & Majaj,
2004). Crowding can be demonstrated even in the absence of impaired target detection (Pelli et al.,
2004) and may be caused by excessive integration of target and distractor representations (Parkes, Lund, Angelucci, Solomon, & Morgan,
2001; Pelli et al.,
2004). Crowding effects are reduced as the target–distractor distance increases, and they disappear entirely when the distractors exceed a specific distance from the target. This distance is labeled the “critical spacing” point. Thus, inside the critical spacing point, target identification improves monotonically as distractor spacing increases. For distractor distances beyond the critical spacing point, target identification is no longer affected by the distractors (Bouma,
1970; Pelli et al.,
2004). Bouma (
1970) determined that critical spacing is roughly 0.5 target eccentricity so that the further in periphery a target is presented, the greater is the critical spacing. Further studies have shown that critical spacing is independent of target and distractor size (Pelli et al.,
2004; Strasburger, Harvey, & Rentschler,
1991), as well as the number of distractors (in cases of two or more) and distractor contrast (Pelli et al.,
2004). Some have suggested that crowding is intimately related to the construct of attention, in that the spatial extent of the interactions between targets and distractors may be determined by the spatial resolution of attention (e.g., Intriligator & Cavanagh,
2001). By this view, closely grouped targets and distractors lead to impaired target discrimination because the resolution of attention is insufficient to disambiguate the relevant and irrelevant elements in the scene.