November 2009
Volume 9, Issue 12
Free
Research Article  |   November 2009
Effect of accommodation on peripheral ocular aberrations
Author Affiliations
Journal of Vision November 2009, Vol.9, 20. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/9.12.20
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Ankit Mathur, David A. Atchison, W. Neil Charman; Effect of accommodation on peripheral ocular aberrations. Journal of Vision 2009;9(12):20. https://doi.org/10.1167/9.12.20.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Changes in peripheral aberrations, particularly higher order aberrations, as a function of accommodation have received little attention. Wavefront aberrations were measured for the right eyes of 9 young adult emmetropes at 38 field positions in the central 42 × 32 degrees of the visual field. Subjects accommodated monocularly to targets at vergences of either 0.3 or 4.0 D. Wavefront data for a 5-mm diameter pupil were analyzed either in terms of the vector components of refraction or Zernike coefficients and total RMS wavefront aberrations. Relative peripheral refractive error (RPRE) was myopic at both accommodation demands and showed only a slight, not statistically significant, hypermetropic shift in the vertical meridian with the higher accommodation demand. There was little change in the astigmatic components of refraction or the higher order Zernike coefficients, apart from fourth-order spherical aberration, which became more negative (by 0.10 μm) at all field locations. Although it has been suggested that nearwork and the state of peripheral refraction may play some role in myopia development, for most of our adult emmetropes any changes with accommodation in RPRE and aberration were small. Hence it seems unlikely that such changes can be of importance to late-onset myopization.

Introduction
Accommodation induces dramatic changes in the optics of the eye, especially in the shape and refractive index distribution of the crystalline lens (e.g., Jones, Atchison, & Pope, 2007; Kasthurirangan, Markwell, Atchison, & Pope, 2008). It has long been known that these changes result in a negative shift in axial (foveal) spherical aberration (Atchison, Collins, Wildsoet, Christensen, & Waterworth, 1995; Cheng et al., 2004; Hazel, Cox, & Strang, 2003; He, Burns, & Marcos, 2000; He, Marcos, Webb, & Burns, 1998; Howland & Buettner, 1989; Koomen, Tousey, & Scolnik, 1949; Ninomiya et al., 2002; Radhakrishnan & Charman, 2007a; Smirnov, 1961). Changes in other aberrations such as coma and astigmatism are not as systematic (Cheng et al., 2004; He et al., 2000; Howland & Buettner, 1989; Lu, Campbell, & Munger, 1994; Plainis, Ginis, & Pallikaris, 2005; Radhakrishnan & Charman, 2007a, 2007b). 
In the peripheral visual field, most studies have reported Zernike aberrations for relaxed accommodation and have been largely limited to the horizontal visual field (Atchison, 2006; Atchison & Scott, 2002; Lundström, Gustafsson, & Unsbo, 2009; Navarro, Moreno, & Dorronsoro, 1998) or to a few locations in the visual field (Lundström, Unsbo, & Gustafsson, 2005; Sheehan, Goncharov, O'Dwyer, Toal, & Dainty, 2007). Aberrations with relaxed accommodation increase away from the center of the visual field and are dominated by defocus and astigmatism (Atchison, 2006; Atchison & Scott, 2002; Guirao & Artal, 1999; Lundström, Gustafsson et al., 2009; Navarro et al., 1998). Mathur et al. (Mathur, Atchison, & Charman, 2009; Mathur, Atchison, & Scott, 2008) have recently extended these distance measurements to cover 42° × 32° of the central visual field. 
There have also been numerous studies of the variation in refractive error across the visual field when accommodation is relaxed (see Atchison & Smith, 2000 for review of earlier work). For the present purposes, the most interesting is the longitudinal study by Hoogerheide, Rempt, and Hoogenboom (1971) who found that the pattern of peripheral refraction differed between different adult refractive groups and that relative peripheral hyperopia (i.e., a positive rather than a negative relative peripheral refractive error, RPRE) tended to favor the development of myopia in originally hyperopic or emmetropic young adults. In a study of groups of existing adult ametropes, Millodot (1981) showed that, at least for field angles up to about 30°, both astigmatic image surfaces in hyperopic eyes along the horizontal meridian usually showed relative peripheral myopia with respect to the axial refraction, whereas in myopic eyes there was relative hyperopia; in emmetropes the two astigmatic image surfaces tended to lie on opposite sides of the retina. Somewhat similar results have since been obtained by several other authors (Atchison, Pritchard, & Schmid, 2006; Love, Gilmartin, & Dunne, 2000; Lundström, Gustafsson et al., 2009; Mutti, Sholtz, Friedman, & Zadnik, 2000; Seidemann, Schaeffel, Guirao, López-Gil, & Artal, 2002), some of whose work also shows differences between the patterns of refraction in different meridians. 
The few studies of the changes in peripheral refraction with accommodation, mainly along the horizontal meridian, gave different results. Smith, Millodot, and McBrien (1988) found that astigmatism at large field angles (>40°) tended to increase with accommodation, almost entirely as a result of the tangential power error becoming increasingly negative, so that the focus in the periphery became relatively more myopic with respect to the axial focus as the accommodation demand increased (i.e., the RPRE became more negative, or myopic). In contrast, Walker and Mutti (2002) found that accommodation to a 3 D stimulus initially caused the peripheral refraction (spherical equivalent) at 30° nasal visual field to become relatively more hyperopic with respect to the axial refraction but that this shift decreased if near accommodation was maintained over a period of 2 h. They suggested that this was associated with longer term changes in the shape of the retina rather than with the optical components of the eye. In a more comprehensive study, confined to myopes in the range −0.5 to −4.5 D, Whatham et al. (2009) determined that the positive RPRE found at distance tended to decrease with accommodation (i.e., the peripheral refraction became relatively more myopic), although effects differed in the nasal and temporal semi-meridians. The regular astigmatism vector J180 tended to become more negative particularly at larger field angles (40°), while oblique astigmatic vector J45 underwent little change. Although Calver, Radhakrishnan, Osuobeni, and O'Leary (2007) found that J180 and overall cylindrical power over the central ±30° tended to decrease with a 2.5-D accommodation stimulus, particularly in the nasal field, their overall conclusion was that “viewing distance has little general effect on peripheral refraction”. Lundström, Mira-Agudelo, and Artal (2009) measured aberrations out to ±40° horizontally and ±20° vertically for 6 emmetropes and 6 myopes and two different states of accommodation (targets at 0.5 D and 4.0 D). The emmetropic group had the expected positive C20 (defocus) coefficient (corresponding to negative RPRE) for the lower accommodation stimulus and this became slightly more positive for the higher stimulus. The low negative C20 (corresponding to positive RPRE) of the myopic group was little affected by accommodation. It may be that the differences between these various studies can be explained partly by the different ranges of accommodation stimulus and field positions employed. 
There is only one previous study on changes in the higher order aberrations with accommodation in the peripheral visual field. While their emphasis was on the second-order coefficients, Lundström, Mira-Agudelo et al. (2009) noted statistically significant effects of accommodation on some higher order aberration coefficients. 
In this paper, we extend previous investigations of the influence of accommodation on central aberrations to the measurement of aberrations across the central visual field. The study was restricted to young adult emmetropes, in an attempt to ensure homogeneous results, and the vergence of the near accommodative stimulus approximated to that of typical real-life near tasks. 
Methods
We recruited 9 young (mean age: 25 years, age range: 21–30 years) emmetropic volunteers (mean and standard deviation of spherical equivalent refraction: 0.2 D ± 0.3 D). Right eyes were assessed, while left eyes were occluded during measurements. Subjects were screened for ocular pathology. All subjects had visual acuities ≥6/6 or better and <0.75 D of central astigmatism. 
Peripheral aberrations were measured using a COAS-HD Hartmann–Shack aberrometer (Wavefront Sciences, Albuquerque, USA) across 38 targets arranged in a 6 row × 7 column matrix, covering 42° × 32° of the central visual field. For each measurement, subjects placed their heads on the instrument's chin rest and sequentially fixated the targets. Two measurements were taken at each field point and their aberration coefficients were averaged. The center of the target matrix was aligned with the instrument's internal fixation target. The pupil center was aligned with the instrument's measurement axis and the cornea was made conjugate to the lenslet array prior to each measurement, using the instrument's alignment camera. A detailed description of the methods has been given previously (Mathur et al., 2008). For 0.3-D accommodative demand, 0.3° black crosses were placed on a white wall 3.0 m from the eye; the luminance of the wall as viewed through the beam splitter was 10 cd/m2. For 4.0-D accommodative demand, 0.5° red spots were projected on a back-projection screen 0.25 m from the eye; the luminances of the red lights as viewed through the beam splitter were 12 cd/m2. Since the screen was flat, the accommodation demand varied slightly across its area, from a maximum of 4.0 D on axis to a minimum of 3.71 D at the largest field angle used (22°). Compensation was made for this by altering the spherical equivalent refraction, in diopters, for the 4.0-D demand by 3.6[cos(ϕ) − 1], where ϕ is the field angle and 3.6 D is the mean accommodation response of the subjects (see below). Prior to presenting targets for the two accommodative demands, the internal target of the COAS-HD was fogged by 1.5 D for axial measurements at 0-D accommodative demand. The difference between the axial spherical equivalent refraction for a given accommodative demand and for 0-D accommodative demand, with sign reversed, was taken as the accommodative response. 
When measuring peripheral aberrations of the eye, the elliptical shape of the pupil must be taken into account. Zernike coefficients for the elliptical pupils were estimated using a Matlab-based algorithm, which assumes that the minor axis of the pupil shortens by the cosine of the off-axis angle along a given meridian. The algorithm expands the pupil along its minor axis to form a circular pupil and estimates Zernike coefficients for the resultant circular pupil (Atchison, Scott, & Charman, 2007). Zernike coefficients up to 6th order for 555-nm wavelength as per the ANSI and ISO standards were estimated for 5.0-mm pupils (ANSI, 2004; ISO, 2008). The room illumination was reduced to ensure that the pupil diameter was at least 5.0 mm with 4.0-D accommodative demand, when any accommodative miosis was maximal. Spherical equivalent refraction M, with/against-the-rule astigmatism J180 and oblique astigmatism J45 were calculated from second, fourth, and sixth Zernike-order coefficients, as described by Atchison, Scott, and Charman (2007, 2008). 
The contour plots representing the magnitude of aberrations at each visual field location were generated using triangle-based interpolation. 
Results
The mean accommodative responses for 0.3-D and 4.0-D accommodative demands were 0.0 D ± 0.3 D and 3.6 D ± 0.6 D, respectively. 
The refraction components and 3rd- to 4th-order Zernike aberration coefficients were analyzed by repeated measures analyses of variance for the variables of accommodation demand (2 states) and field position (38 positions). Table 1 shows the results. The refraction components are oblique astigmatism J 45, relative peripheral refractive error (RPRE), which is the change in M relative to axial M, and with/against-the-rule astigmatism J 180. Results for the higher order root-mean-squared aberrations (HORMS) and the total root-mean-squared aberrations except for defocus (Total RMS) are also shown. Coefficients C 3 1 and C 4 0 and Total RMS were affected significantly by accommodation demand. The refraction components and nearly all the coefficients were affected significantly by field position, and most of them had significant accommodation-field position interactions. 
Table 1
 
The p-values of repeated measures analyses of variance for refraction components, Zernike aberration coefficients and root-mean-squared aberrations for the variables of accommodation demand and field position. The defocus coefficient C 20 is relative to its central field for each accommodation demand but has a correction for the slightly changing accommodation demand with field position as described in the Methods section for RPRE. Asterisks indicate effects which are significant at the 5% level.
Table 1
 
The p-values of repeated measures analyses of variance for refraction components, Zernike aberration coefficients and root-mean-squared aberrations for the variables of accommodation demand and field position. The defocus coefficient C 20 is relative to its central field for each accommodation demand but has a correction for the slightly changing accommodation demand with field position as described in the Methods section for RPRE. Asterisks indicate effects which are significant at the 5% level.
Aberration coefficient Accommodation demand Field position Accommodation × field position interaction
J 45 0.58 0.00* 0.10
RPRE 0.33 0.00* 0.17
J 180 0.22 0.00* 0.00*
C 2 −2 0.98 0.00* 0.00*
C 2 0 0.13 0.00* 0.00*
C 2 2 0.28 0.00* 0.00*
C 3 −3 0.63 0.00* 0.46
C 3 −1 0.65 0.00* 0.00*
C 3 1 0.04* 0.00* 0.00*
C 3 3 0.38 0.00* 0.00*
C 4 −4 0.15 0.00* 0.00*
C 4 −2 0.67 0.84 0.64
C 4 0 0.00* 0.60 0.99
C 4 2 0.35 0.00* 0.00*
C 4 4 0.92 0.00* 0.00*
HORMS 0.43 0.02* 0.99
Total RMS 0.02* 0.00* 0.00*
Figure 1 shows the means of the 9 emmetropes' refractive components: (a) J 45, (b) RPRE, and (c) J 180 at (A) 0.3 D and (B) 4.0 D accommodative demands across the visual field. Figure 1C shows the differences between mean results at the two accommodative demands (B − A). 
Figure 1
 
Mean refractive components, (a) oblique astigmatism J 45, (b) spherical equivalent M, expressed as relative peripheral refractive error ( RPRE), and (c) with/against-the-rule astigmatism J 180 at (A) 0.3-D and (B) 4.0-D accommodative demands across the visual field. (C) The color scales represent the magnitudes in diopters and are the same for all refractive components at both accommodative demands. Differences in J 45, RPRE and J 180 between the two accommodative demands (B − A); the color scales are the same for all refractive components. S, I, N and T represent superior, inferior, nasal and temporal visual fields. Pupil size is 5 mm.
Figure 1
 
Mean refractive components, (a) oblique astigmatism J 45, (b) spherical equivalent M, expressed as relative peripheral refractive error ( RPRE), and (c) with/against-the-rule astigmatism J 180 at (A) 0.3-D and (B) 4.0-D accommodative demands across the visual field. (C) The color scales represent the magnitudes in diopters and are the same for all refractive components at both accommodative demands. Differences in J 45, RPRE and J 180 between the two accommodative demands (B − A); the color scales are the same for all refractive components. S, I, N and T represent superior, inferior, nasal and temporal visual fields. Pupil size is 5 mm.
The RPRE became significantly more negative ( p < 0.001), or myopic, as the peripheral field angle increased for both accommodative demands ( Figures 1Ab, 1Bb, and 2). This shift appeared less marked for the 4.0-D demand than for the 0.3-D demand ( Figures 1Cb and 2), but this difference was not significant when analyzed across the field or along the vertical and horizontal meridians ( Table 1, Figure 2). J 45 ( Figures 1Aa and 1Ba) and J 180 ( Figures 1Ac and 1Bc) did not demonstrate any obvious changes in pattern or magnitude with changes in accommodation ( Table 1). The pattern of variation of J 45 and J 180, primarily along the 45/135 oblique and 90/180 meridians, respectively, is that expected for astigmatism with local axes oriented symmetrically toward a point slightly temporally displaced from the center of the field. 
Figure 2
 
RPRE along (a) the vertical visual field meridian and (b) the horizontal visual field, for 0.3-D and 4.0-D accommodative demands. Error bars are standard deviations and lines are quadratic fits. Plots are staggered horizontally to make them more legible. As there were no measurements along the horizontal visual field, results were obtained by averaging results at vertical field angles of ±3.3°. A second-order fit was made to each subject's results along each meridian for each of the two accommodation demands. Paired t-tests using the second-order coefficients of the fits did not show significant effect of accommodation on RPRE along either the vertical ( p = 0.41) or horizontal meridians ( p = 0.14).
Figure 2
 
RPRE along (a) the vertical visual field meridian and (b) the horizontal visual field, for 0.3-D and 4.0-D accommodative demands. Error bars are standard deviations and lines are quadratic fits. Plots are staggered horizontally to make them more legible. As there were no measurements along the horizontal visual field, results were obtained by averaging results at vertical field angles of ±3.3°. A second-order fit was made to each subject's results along each meridian for each of the two accommodation demands. Paired t-tests using the second-order coefficients of the fits did not show significant effect of accommodation on RPRE along either the vertical ( p = 0.41) or horizontal meridians ( p = 0.14).
The overall patterns of change between the 2 stimulus demands can be more readily appreciated in Figure 1C, which shows the differences between the values of the astigmatic power components and the RPRE at the two accommodation demands (B − A). There was a hyperopic (positive) change in the RPRE with accommodation in the inferior field of up to 0.3 D, but as mentioned above, this shift was not significant. Changes in the two astigmatic components were small. 
Figures 3A and 3B show mean higher order elliptical wavefront maps across the pupil for each visual field location for 0.3-D and 4.0-D accommodative demands, respectively. With second-order aberrations excluded, the main peripheral aberrations appear to be 3rd-order coma and 4th-order spherical aberration. The combination of vertical and horizontal coma dominated the visual field at 0.3-D accommodative demand, with an orientation that was approximately radial with respect to the field center, but negative spherical aberration became relatively more important at 4-D demand. 
Figure 3
 
Mean higher order elliptical wavefront maps at each visual field location for (A) 0.3-D and (B) 4.0-D accommodative demands. Aberrations in the third to sixth Zernike orders are included. The minor axis of the elliptical wavefront maps is cosine of visual field angle times the major axis. I, N, S and T represent inferior, nasal, superior and temporal visual fields. Pupil size is 5 mm.
Figure 3
 
Mean higher order elliptical wavefront maps at each visual field location for (A) 0.3-D and (B) 4.0-D accommodative demands. Aberrations in the third to sixth Zernike orders are included. The minor axis of the elliptical wavefront maps is cosine of visual field angle times the major axis. I, N, S and T represent inferior, nasal, superior and temporal visual fields. Pupil size is 5 mm.
Figures 4A and 4B shows some mean higher order aberration coefficients, HORMS and Total RMS across the visual field for 0.3-D and 4.0-D accommodative demands, respectively. Figure 4C shows the differences between mean results at the two accommodative demands (B − A). Third-order aberrations showed little change between the two accommodation demands, although horizontal coma coefficient C 3 1 was affected significantly by accommodation demand ( Table 1). Oblique trefoil coefficient C 3 −3 increased linearly from the inferior to the superior visual field ( Figures 4Aa and 4Ba), vertical coma coefficient C 3 −1 increased linearly from the superior to the inferior visual field ( Figures 4Ab and 4Bb), and horizontal coma coefficient C 3 1 increased linearly from the nasal to the temporal visual field ( Figures 4Ac and 4Bc). The spherical aberration coefficient C 4 0 had a significant shift of −0.10 ± 0.04 μm ( p < 0.001) across the field with increase in accommodative demand, from a mean value of about +0.03 μm to −0.06 μm (compare Figure 4Ad with Figure 4Bd), but it did not show any pattern across the visual field for either accommodation demand ( Table 1). HORMS increased slightly with field angle ( Figures 4Ae and 4Be). Total RMS also increased from the center of the visual field; the increase with field angle was slightly larger for the 0.3-D demand ( Figure 4Af) than for 4-D demand ( Figure 4Bf; Table 1). 
Figure 4
 
Mean individual higher order aberration terms across the visual field at (A) 0.3-D and (B) 4.0-D accommodative demands. (a) Oblique trefoil C 3 −3, (b) vertical coma C 3 −1, (c) horizontal coma C 3 1, (d) spherical aberration C 4 0, (e) higher order root-mean-square aberrations ( HORMS) and (f) total root-mean-square aberrations excluding defocus ( Total RMS). The color scales represent the magnitude of aberration coefficient in micrometers (μm) and are same for a given aberration at both accommodative demands. (C) The difference between the values of the terms at the two accommodation demands (B − A); the color scales are the same for all aberration terms and HORMS. S, I, N and T represent superior, inferior, nasal and temporal visual fields. Pupil size is 5 mm.
Figure 4
 
Mean individual higher order aberration terms across the visual field at (A) 0.3-D and (B) 4.0-D accommodative demands. (a) Oblique trefoil C 3 −3, (b) vertical coma C 3 −1, (c) horizontal coma C 3 1, (d) spherical aberration C 4 0, (e) higher order root-mean-square aberrations ( HORMS) and (f) total root-mean-square aberrations excluding defocus ( Total RMS). The color scales represent the magnitude of aberration coefficient in micrometers (μm) and are same for a given aberration at both accommodative demands. (C) The difference between the values of the terms at the two accommodation demands (B − A); the color scales are the same for all aberration terms and HORMS. S, I, N and T represent superior, inferior, nasal and temporal visual fields. Pupil size is 5 mm.
It is again helpful to study the changes by subtracting the two sets of aberration maps from one another (i.e., wavefront aberration for 4.0-D stimulus − wavefront aberration for 0.3-D stimulus). The results confirm that most of the aberration terms showed only small changes ( Figure 4C). The exception was C 4 0, for there was an almost uniform change of −0.10 μm across the field ( Figure 4Cd). 
The use of mean data in preceding figures may conceal marked inter-subject variations. However, examination of plots for individual subjects indicated that most followed the same broad pattern as the mean data. The changes in coma coefficients across the visual field with change in accommodation were further analyzed. The slopes of coma coefficients, along vertical and horizontal visual field meridians, for the two accommodative demands were compared using paired t-tests. Figures 5a and 5b show mean vertical coma coefficient C 3 −1 and mean horizontal coma coefficient C 3 1 across vertical and horizontal visual field meridians, respectively, for 0.3-D and 4.0-D accommodative demands. Although the coma slopes were slightly greater at the lower than at the higher accommodative demand, the differences were not significant. Note that the plots in Figure 5 pass close to the origin and are very similar in the two meridians, implying that total coma tends to increase with field angle and that its orientation is essentially radial. 
Figure 5
 
(a) Vertical coma coefficients C 3 −1 and (b) horizontal C 3 1 coefficients along vertical and horizontal visual field meridians, for 0.3-D and 4.0-D accommodative demands. Error bars are standard deviations and lines are linear fits. Plots are staggered horizontally to make them more legible. As there were no measurements along the horizontal visual field, horizontal coma for the horizontal visual field was obtained by averaging results at vertical field angles of ±3.3°. A linear fit for horizontal coma was made to each subject's results along the horizontal meridian for each of the two accommodation demands. Similarly, linear fits were made for vertical coma along the vertical meridian. Paired t-tests using the slopes of the fits did not show significant effects of accommodation on coma along either the vertical ( p = 0.11) or horizontal meridians ( p = 0.37).
Figure 5
 
(a) Vertical coma coefficients C 3 −1 and (b) horizontal C 3 1 coefficients along vertical and horizontal visual field meridians, for 0.3-D and 4.0-D accommodative demands. Error bars are standard deviations and lines are linear fits. Plots are staggered horizontally to make them more legible. As there were no measurements along the horizontal visual field, horizontal coma for the horizontal visual field was obtained by averaging results at vertical field angles of ±3.3°. A linear fit for horizontal coma was made to each subject's results along the horizontal meridian for each of the two accommodation demands. Similarly, linear fits were made for vertical coma along the vertical meridian. Paired t-tests using the slopes of the fits did not show significant effects of accommodation on coma along either the vertical ( p = 0.11) or horizontal meridians ( p = 0.37).
Discussion
In this study, peripheral aberrations were determined across the central 42° × 32° of the visual field for 0.3-D and 4.0-D accommodation stimuli. At both accommodation demands RPRE was mostly myopic. RPRE showed a slight positive (hypermetropic) shift at the higher accommodation demand in the inferior and superior field, but this shape change was not significant ( Figure 2). The astigmatic components of refraction and most higher order Zernike coefficients, except 4th-order spherical aberration, changed little across the field with accommodation ( Figures 1 and 4, Table 1). The increase in accommodation caused a significant negative shift (by 0.10 μm) at all field locations in 4th-order spherical aberration. 
The aberrational data have some limitations. First, they cover only a field of radius of approximately 20°, whereas measurements of peripheral astigmatism suggest that the largest changes with accommodation occur at field angles of 30° and more (Smith et al., 1988; Whatham et al., 2009). On the other hand, sensitivity to defocus as evidenced by depth-of-focus (Wang & Ciuffreda, 2004; Wang, Ciuffreda, & Irish, 2006) or accommodation response (Bullimore & Gilmartin, 1987; Gu & Legge, 1987) declines quite rapidly with field angle over the central 10° radius of visual field, due largely to the fall-off in cone and ganglion cell density (Jonas, Schneider, & Naumann, 1992), so that, if retinal control of eye growth as a result of local defocus were to follow a similar pattern, this constraint may not be serious. Second, our measurements rely on subjects maintaining the same level of accommodation during the lengthy series of measurements at the different field angles: this would be expected to have greatest effect on the stability of the values of the spherical equivalent M. We attempted to minimize this problem by allowing subjects to have breaks on request and at least once during each measurement series. Third, our analysis is in terms of the values of the accommodation stimulus demands, rather than response levels. The actual values of response differed somewhat between subjects. However, examination of the individual data suggested that all the response changes between the 2 stimulus demands were broadly similar (mean and SD 3.6 ± 0.5 D). Fourth, our subjects were emmetropes, all of whom had essentially myopic RPRE for the 0.3-D accommodation stimulus. It remains possible that, for example, subjects with hyperopic RPRE might have displayed more striking changes in aberration. Fifth (as in most earlier studies), subjects maintained accommodation for only relatively short periods of time. It may be that during long periods of nearwork larger aberrational changes might occur, either as a result of corneal change (Buehren, Collins, & Carney, 2003, 2005) or change in the shape of the globe (Mutti et al., 2007). Lastly, we compared the two accommodation demands at a common pupil size, whereas pupil size decreases linearly with increase in accommodation demand at constant illumination (e.g., Kasthurirangan & Glasser, 2005). Thus it must be appreciated that if the unaccommodated pupil size was actually 5 mm, the aberrations in the accommodated case would be smaller than shown here and have different effects on retinal imagery. The rate of change of coma would reduce across the field and the negative spherical aberration would be smaller. As our actual pupil sizes all exceeded 5 mm, the pupil centers were likely to be slightly inaccurate for a 5-mm pupil (Walsh, 1988; Wilson, Campbell, & Simonet, 1992) and this would affect aberrations. However, shifts in pupil center, because of the differences in pupil size with accommodation, are allowed for in the analysis of the Shack–Hartmann patterns and are hence taken account of in the results. 
Within these constraints, the data suggest that, across the measured field, RPRE for emmetropes changes little with accommodation (see Figures 1Cb and 2). This accords with the findings of earlier authors (Calver et al., 2007; Lundström, Mira-Agudelo et al., 2009; Smith et al., 1988), who found only small changes for field angles of 20° or less. Among the higher order Zernike coefficients, spherical aberration was the only coefficient that changed both significantly and markedly with accommodation. For this coefficient the average off-axis changes were similar to those occurring on axis. Total higher order RMS aberrations were slightly lower with accommodation than at distance. 
In this study, the off-axis Zernike coefficients were calculated over an elliptical pupil by expanding it along its minor axis to form a circle. An alternate approach would be to use a circular pupil with the same diameter as the major axis of the elliptical pupil (e.g., Lundström, Gustafsson et al., 2009; Lundström, Mira-Agudelo et al., 2009). We verified that this gave slightly larger absolute coefficients than those reported here but did not affect the conclusions. 
Are these results of relevance to our understanding of myopia development? Myopia is an increasing problem in many parts of the world (Zadnik & Mutti, 1998) and it has long been hypothesized that prolonged periods of nearwork encourage its development (Cohn, 1886). The axial growth and myopia produced in chicks and other animals fitted with negative lenses to place the image behind the retina (see, e.g., Norton, 1999; Smith, 1998; Wildsoet, 1997 for reviews) led to the suggestion that a similar role could be played by accommodative lag, or under-accommodation, during nearwork (Abbott, Schmid, & Strang, 1998; Allen & O'Leary, 2006; Gwiazda, Thorn, Bauer, & Held, 1993). However it now appears that larger lags in accommodation accompany the development of myopia in children, rather than preceding it (Mutti et al., 2006). This has caused renewed interest in an alternative suggestion made by Hoogerheide et al. (1971): in at-risk individuals, relative hyperopia in the retinal periphery precipitates axial eye growth and myopia (Charman, 2005; Seidemann et al., 2002; Stone & Flitcroft, 2004; Wallman & Winawer, 2004). This concept has received some support from a longitudinal study in children (Mutti et al., 2007) and several animal studies (Diether & Schaeffel, 1997; Hung, Ramamirtham, Huang, Qiao-Grider, & Smith, 2008; Smith, Hung, & Huang, 2009; Smith, Hung, Ramamirtham, Huang, & Qiao-Grider, 2007; Smith, Kee, Ramamirtham, Qiao-Grider, & Hung, 2005; Smith, Ramamirtham et al., 2007). Although in its simple form this mechanism does not necessarily involve nearwork and accommodation, it would follow that if accommodation increased relative peripheral hyperopia, or perhaps image blur as a result of increased aberration, myopia development would become more likely. 
Overall, the present results over the central field of a relatively small group ( N = 9) of young adult emmetropes do not support the concept that accommodation increases hyperopic RPRE or higher order aberrational blur. They thus make it unlikely that late-onset myopia in adult emmetropes is triggered by nearwork-induced changes in RPRE or aberration. However, they do not exclude the potential importance of accommodative lags or the possibility that other specific at-risk individuals or refractive groups (Lundström, Mira-Agudelo et al., 2009) might show different patterns of change in RPRE and aberration. Hoogerheide et al. (1971) found in their longitudinal study that only about 10% of their initially emmetropic adult subjects had a hyperopic RPRE in one or both horizontal semi-meridians and that, of these, only about half developed myopia, so that studies on larger subject groups than that used in the present study may be required to ensure that such possibly at-risk individuals are included. 
Acknowledgments
Neil Charman was supported by the Australian Research Council International Linkage Fellowship LX0881907. 
Commercial relationships: none. 
Corresponding author: Ankit Mathur. 
Email: a.mathur@qut.edu.au. 
Address: School of Optometry and Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia. 
References
Abbott, M. L. Schmid, K. L. Strang, N. C. (1998). Differences in the accommodation stimulus response curve of adult myopes and emmetropes. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 18, 13–20. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Allen, P. M. O'Leary, D. J. (2006). Accommodation functions: Co-dependency and relationship to refractive error. Vision Research, 46, 491–505. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
(2004). American National Standards Institute. American National Standard for Ophthalmics—Methods for reporting optical aberrations of the eye. ANSI Z80.28-2004. : American National Standards Institute.
Atchison, D. A. (2006). Higher order aberrations across the horizontal visual field. Journal of Biomedical Optics, 11, 034026. [CrossRef]
Atchison, D. A. Collins, M. J. Wildsoet, C. F. Christensen, J. Waterworth, M. D. (1995). Measurement of monochromatic ocular aberrations of human eyes as a function of accommodation by the Howland aberroscope technique. Vision Research, 35, 313–323. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Atchison, D. A. Pritchard, N. Schmid, K. L. (2006). Peripheral refraction along the horizontal and vertical visual fields in myopia. Vision Research, 46, 1450–1458. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Atchison, D. A. Scott, D. H. (2002). Monochromatic aberrations of human eyes in the horizontal visual field. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 19, 2180–2184. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Atchison, D. A. Scott, D. H. Charman, W. N. (2007). Measuring ocular aberrations in the peripheral visual field using Hartmann–Shack aberrometry. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 24, 2963–2973. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Atchison, D. A. Scott, D. H. Charman, W. N. (2008). Measuring ocular aberrations in the peripheral visual field using Hartmann–Shack aberrometry: Errata. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 25, 2467. [CrossRef]
Atchison, D. A. Smith, G. (2000). Optics of the human eye. (pp. 147–150). Oxford, UK: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Buehren, T. Collins, M. J. Carney, L. G. (2003). Corneal aberrations and reading. Optometry and Vision Science, 80, 159–166. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Buehren, T. Collins, M. J. Carney, L. G. (2005). Near work induced wavefront aberrations in myopia. Vision Research, 45, 1297–1312. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bullimore, M. A. Gilmartin, B. (1987). Retinal eccentricity and the accommodative response. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics, 64, 644–645. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Calver, R. Radhakrishnan, H. Osuobeni, E. O'Leary, D. (2007). Peripheral refraction for distance and near vision in emmetropes and myopes. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 27, 584–593. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Charman, W. N. (2005). Aberrations and myopia. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 25, 285–301. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cheng, H. Barnett, J. K. Vilupuru, A. S. Marsack, J. D. Kasthurirangan, S. Applegate, R. A. (2004). A population study on changes in wave aberrations with accommodation. Journal of Vision, 4, (4):3, 272–280, http://journalofvision.org/4/4/3/, doi:10.1167/4.4.3. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Cohn, H. (1886). The hygiene of the eye in schools. London: Simpkin, Marshall & Co.
Diether, S. Schaeffel, F. (1997). Local changes in eye growth induced by imposed local refractive error despite active accommodation. Vision Research, 37, 659–668. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gu, Y. C. Legge, G. E. (1987). Accommodation to stimuli in peripheral vision. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics and Image Science, 4, 1681–1687. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Guirao, A. Artal, P. (1999). Off-axis monochromatic aberrations estimated from double pass measurements in the human eye. Vision Research, 39, 207–217. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gwiazda, J. Thorn, F. Bauer, J. Held, R. (1993). Myopic children show insufficient accommodative response to blur. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 34, 690–694. [PubMed] [Article] [PubMed]
Hazel, C. Cox, M. J. Strang, N. (2003). Wavefront aberration and its relationship to the accommodative stimulus-response function in myopic subjects. Optometry and Vision Science, 80, 151–158. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
He, J. C. Burns, S. A. Marcos, S. (2000). Monochromatic aberrations in the accommodated human eye. Vision Research, 40, 41–48. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
He, J. C. Marcos, S. Webb, R. H. Burns, S. A. (1998). Measurement of the wave-front aberration of the eye by a fast psychophysical procedure. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 15, 2449–2456. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hoogerheide, J. Rempt, F. Hoogenboom, W. P. H. (1971). Acquired myopia in young pilots. Ophthalmologica, 163, 209–215. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Howland, H. C. Buettner, J. (1989). Computing high order wave aberration coefficients from variations of best focus for small artificial pupils. Vision Research, 29, 979–983. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hung, L. F. Ramamirtham, R. Huang, J. Qiao-Grider, Y. Smith, E. L. (2008). Peripheral refraction in normal infant rhesus monkeys. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 49, 3747–3757. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
(2008). International Organization for Standardization. International Organization for Standardization. Ophthalmic optics and instruments—Reporting aberrations of the human eye. ISO 24157:2008.
Jonas, J. B. Schneider, U. Naumann, G. O. (1992). Count and density of human retinal photoreceptors. Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology, 230, 505–510. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Jones, C. E. Atchison, D. A. Pope, J. M. (2007). Changes in lens dimensions and refractive index with age and accommodation. Optometry and Vision Science, 84, 990–995. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kasthurirangan, S. Glasser, A. (2005). Characteristics of pupil responses during far-to-near and near-to-far accommodation. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 25, 328–339. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kasthurirangan, S. Markwell, E. L. Atchison, D. A. Pope, J. M. (2008). In vivo study of changes in refractive index distribution in the human crystalline lens with age and accommodation. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 49, 2531–2540. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Koomen, M. Tousey, R. Scolnik, R. (1949). The spherical aberration of the eye. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 39, 370–376. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Love, J. Gilmartin, B. Dunne, M. C. M. (2000). Relative peripheral refractive error in adult myopia and emmetropia. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 41,
Lu, C. Campbell, M. C. W. Munger, R. (1994). Monochromatic aberrations in accommodated eyes. Ophthalmic and visual optics technical digest. (pp. 160–163). Washington, DC: The Optical Society of America.
Lundström, L. Gustafsson, J. Unsbo, P. (2009). Population distribution of wavefront aberrations in the peripheral human eye. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 26, 2192–2198. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lundström, L. Mira-Agudelo, A. Artal, P. (2009). Peripheral optical errors and their change with accommodation differ between emmetropic and myopic eyes. Journal of Vision, 9, (6):17, 1–11, http://journalofvision.org/9/6/17/, doi:10.1167/9.6.17. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lundström, L. Unsbo, P. Gustafsson, J. (2005). Off-axis wave front measurements for optical correction in eccentric viewing. Journal of Biomedical Optics, 10, 034002. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mathur, A. Atchison, D. A. Charman, W. N. (2009). Myopia and peripheral ocular aberrations. Journal of Vision, 9, (10):15, 1–12, http://journalofvision.org/9/10/15/, doi:10.1167/9.10.15. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mathur, A. Atchison, D. A. Scott, D. H. (2008). Ocular aberrations in the peripheral visual field. Optics Letters, 33, 863–865. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Millodot, M. (1981). Effect of ametropia on peripheral refraction. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics, 58, 691–695. [PubMed] [PubMed]
Mutti, D. O. Hayes, J. R. Mitchell, G. L. Jones, L. A. Moeschberger, M. L. Cotter, S. A. (2007). Refractive error, axial length, and relative peripheral refractive error before and after the onset of myopia. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 48, 2510–2519. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mutti, D. O. Mitchell, G. L. Hayes, J. R. Jones, L. A. Moeschberger, M. L. Cotter, S. A. (2006). Accommodative lag before and after the onset of myopia. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 47, 837–846. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mutti, D. O. Sholtz, R. I. Friedman, N. E. Zadnik, K. (2000). Peripheral refraction and ocular shape in children. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 41, 1022–1030. [PubMed] [Article] [PubMed]
Navarro, R. Moreno, E. Dorronsoro, C. (1998). Monochromatic aberrations and point-spread functions of the human eye across the visual field. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 15, 2522–2529. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ninomiya, S. Fujikado, T. Kuroda, T. Maeda, N. Tano, Y. Oshika, T. (2002). Changes of ocular aberration with accommodation. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 134, 924–926. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Norton, T. T. (1999). Animal models of myopia: Learning how vision controls the size of the eye. ILAR Journal, 40, 59–77. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Plainis, S. Ginis, H. S. Pallikaris, A. (2005). The effect of ocular aberrations on steady-state errors of accommodative response. Journal of Vision, 5, (5):7, 466–477, http://journalofvision.org/5/5/7/, doi:10.1167/5.5.7. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Radhakrishnan, H. Charman, W. N. (2007a). Age-related changes in ocular aberrations with accommodation. Journal of Vision, 7, (7):11, 1–21, http://journalofvision.org/7/7/11/, doi:10.1167/7.7.11. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef]
Radhakrishnan, H. Charman, W. N. (2007b). Changes in astigmatism with accommodation. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 27, 275–280. [PubMed] [CrossRef]
Seidemann, A. Schaeffel, F. Guirao, A. López-Gil, N. Artal, P. (2002). Peripheral refractive errors in myopic, emmetropic, and hyperopic young subjects. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics, Image Science, and Vision, 19, 2363–2373. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sheehan, M. T. Goncharov, A. V. O'Dwyer, V. M. Toal, V. Dainty, C. (2007). Population study of the variation in monochromatic aberrations of the normal human eye over the central visual field. Optics Express, 15, 7367–7380. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Smirnov, M. S. (1961). Measurement of the wave aberration of the human eye. Biofizika, 6, 776–794. [PubMed] [PubMed]
Smith, E. L. Rosenfield, M. Gilmartin, B. (1998). Environmentally induced refractive error in animals. Myopia and nearwork. (pp. 57–90). Oxford, UK: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Smith, E. L. Hung, L. Huang, J. (2009). Relative peripheral hyperopic defocus alters central refractive development in infant monkeys. Vision Research, 49, 2386–2392. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Smith, E. L. Hung, L. F. Ramamirtham, R. Huang, J. Qiao-Grider, Y. (2007). Optically imposed hyperopic defocus in the periphery can produce central axial myopia in infant monkeys [ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://abstractsiovsorg/cgi/content/abstract/48/5/1533maxtoshow=&HITS=10&hits=10&RESULTFORMAT=1&author1=Smith%2C+EL&andorexacttitle=and&andorexacttitleabs=and&andorexactfulltext=and&searchid=1&FIRSTINDEX=0&sortspec=relevance&fdate=2/1/2006&resourcetype=">Abstract/ext-link>]. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 48, 1533.
Smith, E. L. Kee, C. S. Ramamirtham, R. Qiao-Grider, Y. Hung, L. F. (2005). Peripheral vision can influence eye growth and refractive development in infant monkeys. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 46, 3965–3972. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Smith, E. L. Ramamirtham, R. Qiao-Grider, Y. Hung, L. F. Huang, J. Kee, C. S. (2007). Effects of foveal ablation on emmetropization and form-deprivation myopia. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 48, 3914–3922. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Smith, G. Millodot, M. McBrien, N. (1988). The effect of accommodation on oblique astigmatism and field curvature of the human eye. Clinical and Experimental Optometry, 71, 119–125. [CrossRef]
Stone, R. A. Flitcroft, D. I. (2004). Ocular shape and myopia. Annals of the Academy of Medicine Singapore, 33, 7–15. [PubMed]
Walker, T. W. Mutti, D. O. (2002). The effect of accommodation on ocular shape. Optometry and Vision Science, 79, 424–430. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wallman, J. Winawer, J. (2004). Homeostasis of eye growth and the question of myopia. Neuron, 43, 447–468. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Walsh, G. (1988). The effect of mydriasis on the pupillary centration of the human eye. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 8, 178–182. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wang, B. Ciuffreda, K. J. (2004). Depth-of-focus of the human eye in the near retinal periphery. Vision Research, 44, 1115–1125. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wang, B. Ciuffreda, K. J. Irish, T. (2006). Equiblur zones at the fovea and near retinal periphery. Vision Research, 46, 3690–3698. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Whatham, A. Zimmermann, F. Martinez, A. Delgado, S. de la Jara, P. L. Sankaridurg, P. (2009). Influence of accommodation on off-axis refractive errors in myopic eyes. Journal of Vision, 9, (3):14, 1–13, http://journalofvision.org/9/3/14/, doi:10.1167/9.3.14. [PubMed] [Article] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wildsoet, C. F. (1997). Active emmetropization: Evidence for its existence and ramifications for clinical practice. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 17, 279–290. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wilson, M. A. Campbell, M. C. Simonet, P. (1992). The Julius F Neumueller Award in Optics, 1989: Change of pupil centration with change of illumination and pupil size. Optometry and Vision Science, 69, 129–136. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Zadnik, K. Mutti, D. O. Rosenfield, M. Gilmartin, B. (1998). Prevalence of myopia. Myopia and nearwork. (pp. 13–30). Oxford, UK: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Figure 1
 
Mean refractive components, (a) oblique astigmatism J 45, (b) spherical equivalent M, expressed as relative peripheral refractive error ( RPRE), and (c) with/against-the-rule astigmatism J 180 at (A) 0.3-D and (B) 4.0-D accommodative demands across the visual field. (C) The color scales represent the magnitudes in diopters and are the same for all refractive components at both accommodative demands. Differences in J 45, RPRE and J 180 between the two accommodative demands (B − A); the color scales are the same for all refractive components. S, I, N and T represent superior, inferior, nasal and temporal visual fields. Pupil size is 5 mm.
Figure 1
 
Mean refractive components, (a) oblique astigmatism J 45, (b) spherical equivalent M, expressed as relative peripheral refractive error ( RPRE), and (c) with/against-the-rule astigmatism J 180 at (A) 0.3-D and (B) 4.0-D accommodative demands across the visual field. (C) The color scales represent the magnitudes in diopters and are the same for all refractive components at both accommodative demands. Differences in J 45, RPRE and J 180 between the two accommodative demands (B − A); the color scales are the same for all refractive components. S, I, N and T represent superior, inferior, nasal and temporal visual fields. Pupil size is 5 mm.
Figure 2
 
RPRE along (a) the vertical visual field meridian and (b) the horizontal visual field, for 0.3-D and 4.0-D accommodative demands. Error bars are standard deviations and lines are quadratic fits. Plots are staggered horizontally to make them more legible. As there were no measurements along the horizontal visual field, results were obtained by averaging results at vertical field angles of ±3.3°. A second-order fit was made to each subject's results along each meridian for each of the two accommodation demands. Paired t-tests using the second-order coefficients of the fits did not show significant effect of accommodation on RPRE along either the vertical ( p = 0.41) or horizontal meridians ( p = 0.14).
Figure 2
 
RPRE along (a) the vertical visual field meridian and (b) the horizontal visual field, for 0.3-D and 4.0-D accommodative demands. Error bars are standard deviations and lines are quadratic fits. Plots are staggered horizontally to make them more legible. As there were no measurements along the horizontal visual field, results were obtained by averaging results at vertical field angles of ±3.3°. A second-order fit was made to each subject's results along each meridian for each of the two accommodation demands. Paired t-tests using the second-order coefficients of the fits did not show significant effect of accommodation on RPRE along either the vertical ( p = 0.41) or horizontal meridians ( p = 0.14).
Figure 3
 
Mean higher order elliptical wavefront maps at each visual field location for (A) 0.3-D and (B) 4.0-D accommodative demands. Aberrations in the third to sixth Zernike orders are included. The minor axis of the elliptical wavefront maps is cosine of visual field angle times the major axis. I, N, S and T represent inferior, nasal, superior and temporal visual fields. Pupil size is 5 mm.
Figure 3
 
Mean higher order elliptical wavefront maps at each visual field location for (A) 0.3-D and (B) 4.0-D accommodative demands. Aberrations in the third to sixth Zernike orders are included. The minor axis of the elliptical wavefront maps is cosine of visual field angle times the major axis. I, N, S and T represent inferior, nasal, superior and temporal visual fields. Pupil size is 5 mm.
Figure 4
 
Mean individual higher order aberration terms across the visual field at (A) 0.3-D and (B) 4.0-D accommodative demands. (a) Oblique trefoil C 3 −3, (b) vertical coma C 3 −1, (c) horizontal coma C 3 1, (d) spherical aberration C 4 0, (e) higher order root-mean-square aberrations ( HORMS) and (f) total root-mean-square aberrations excluding defocus ( Total RMS). The color scales represent the magnitude of aberration coefficient in micrometers (μm) and are same for a given aberration at both accommodative demands. (C) The difference between the values of the terms at the two accommodation demands (B − A); the color scales are the same for all aberration terms and HORMS. S, I, N and T represent superior, inferior, nasal and temporal visual fields. Pupil size is 5 mm.
Figure 4
 
Mean individual higher order aberration terms across the visual field at (A) 0.3-D and (B) 4.0-D accommodative demands. (a) Oblique trefoil C 3 −3, (b) vertical coma C 3 −1, (c) horizontal coma C 3 1, (d) spherical aberration C 4 0, (e) higher order root-mean-square aberrations ( HORMS) and (f) total root-mean-square aberrations excluding defocus ( Total RMS). The color scales represent the magnitude of aberration coefficient in micrometers (μm) and are same for a given aberration at both accommodative demands. (C) The difference between the values of the terms at the two accommodation demands (B − A); the color scales are the same for all aberration terms and HORMS. S, I, N and T represent superior, inferior, nasal and temporal visual fields. Pupil size is 5 mm.
Figure 5
 
(a) Vertical coma coefficients C 3 −1 and (b) horizontal C 3 1 coefficients along vertical and horizontal visual field meridians, for 0.3-D and 4.0-D accommodative demands. Error bars are standard deviations and lines are linear fits. Plots are staggered horizontally to make them more legible. As there were no measurements along the horizontal visual field, horizontal coma for the horizontal visual field was obtained by averaging results at vertical field angles of ±3.3°. A linear fit for horizontal coma was made to each subject's results along the horizontal meridian for each of the two accommodation demands. Similarly, linear fits were made for vertical coma along the vertical meridian. Paired t-tests using the slopes of the fits did not show significant effects of accommodation on coma along either the vertical ( p = 0.11) or horizontal meridians ( p = 0.37).
Figure 5
 
(a) Vertical coma coefficients C 3 −1 and (b) horizontal C 3 1 coefficients along vertical and horizontal visual field meridians, for 0.3-D and 4.0-D accommodative demands. Error bars are standard deviations and lines are linear fits. Plots are staggered horizontally to make them more legible. As there were no measurements along the horizontal visual field, horizontal coma for the horizontal visual field was obtained by averaging results at vertical field angles of ±3.3°. A linear fit for horizontal coma was made to each subject's results along the horizontal meridian for each of the two accommodation demands. Similarly, linear fits were made for vertical coma along the vertical meridian. Paired t-tests using the slopes of the fits did not show significant effects of accommodation on coma along either the vertical ( p = 0.11) or horizontal meridians ( p = 0.37).
Table 1
 
The p-values of repeated measures analyses of variance for refraction components, Zernike aberration coefficients and root-mean-squared aberrations for the variables of accommodation demand and field position. The defocus coefficient C 20 is relative to its central field for each accommodation demand but has a correction for the slightly changing accommodation demand with field position as described in the Methods section for RPRE. Asterisks indicate effects which are significant at the 5% level.
Table 1
 
The p-values of repeated measures analyses of variance for refraction components, Zernike aberration coefficients and root-mean-squared aberrations for the variables of accommodation demand and field position. The defocus coefficient C 20 is relative to its central field for each accommodation demand but has a correction for the slightly changing accommodation demand with field position as described in the Methods section for RPRE. Asterisks indicate effects which are significant at the 5% level.
Aberration coefficient Accommodation demand Field position Accommodation × field position interaction
J 45 0.58 0.00* 0.10
RPRE 0.33 0.00* 0.17
J 180 0.22 0.00* 0.00*
C 2 −2 0.98 0.00* 0.00*
C 2 0 0.13 0.00* 0.00*
C 2 2 0.28 0.00* 0.00*
C 3 −3 0.63 0.00* 0.46
C 3 −1 0.65 0.00* 0.00*
C 3 1 0.04* 0.00* 0.00*
C 3 3 0.38 0.00* 0.00*
C 4 −4 0.15 0.00* 0.00*
C 4 −2 0.67 0.84 0.64
C 4 0 0.00* 0.60 0.99
C 4 2 0.35 0.00* 0.00*
C 4 4 0.92 0.00* 0.00*
HORMS 0.43 0.02* 0.99
Total RMS 0.02* 0.00* 0.00*
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×