Sensitivity to direction, velocity, and orientation of luminosity are found as early as the primary visual cortex (Hubel & Wiesel,
1959,
1962; Movshon,
1975). As such, first-order motion signal could be extracted based on the responses of the striate cortex, at which level an oriented spatiotemporal filtering is performed (Dumoulin, Baker, Hess, & Evans,
2003). Therefore, luminance-based motion analysis is thought to occur at lower levels of the visual system, through local integrative processes (Dumoulin et al.,
2003; Smith, Greenlee, Singh, Kraemer, & Hennig,
1998; Wilson et al.,
1992). In contrast, because in second-order stimuli spatiotemporal frequency content is drift-balanced (Chubb & Sperling,
1988; Solomon & Sperling,
1994), second-order motion perception would arise at higher levels of the visual system, possibly at the level of the extrastriate cortex (Dumoulin et al.,
2003; Smith et al.,
1998; Wenderoth, Watson, Egan, Tochon-Danguy, & O'Keefe,
1999). Second-order processing mechanisms are believed to operate through a filter–rectify–filter (Chubb & Sperling,
1988; Wilson et al.,
1992) or position-based analysis (Del Viva & Morrone,
1998; Derrington & Ukkonen,
1999; Lu & Sperling,
1995; Scott-Samuel & Georgeson,
1999b; Seiffert & Cavanagh,
1998; Smith,
1994). Indeed, Seiffert and Cavanagh (
1998) demonstrated that various types of second-order motion stimuli, such as contrast-, orientation- and stereo-defined, are analyzed by position-based mechanisms. Similar results have been obtained by Derrington and Ukkonen (
1999) using contrast-modulated stimuli. These authors demonstrated that second-order mechanisms are sensitive to changes in spatial location of features of the image under motion, using a pedestal motion paradigm (Derrington & Ukkonen,
1999). Although position-based mechanisms or feature-tracking mechanisms are generally considered second-order mechanisms (i.e., not first-order mechanisms), some authors have also classified them as third-order mechanisms (Lu & Sperling,
1995,
2001). In the present paper, we will not make the distinction between second- and third-order properties, as this is not the essence of our study. Rather, we attempt to determine whether the perception of fractal rotation has characteristics that abide to higher order properties. As with most previous authors, we will therefore refer to second-order mechanisms in this broader sense throughout the paper.