The RMS error measures the total performance error both visually and non-visually driven. The errors specific to the perturbation frequencies, however, are a better measure of the visually driven response specific to the moving line, and Fourier analysis allows us to segregate response-gain and response-delay effects. To analyze how participants' control response varied across color-feedback types at each of the input line perturbation frequencies, we computed the human operator transfer function (i.e., the ratio of the Fourier transform of the joystick displacement to that of the visual position error, see
Figure 2).
Figure 6 plots the mean response gain, averaged across the six trials and seven participants, as a function of frequency for each of the color-feedback and control dynamics conditions. We excluded the gains from the lowest three frequencies (<0.1 Hz) because our performance measurements were noisy at these ultra-low frequencies, due to the limited number of cycles available for analysis. The mean response gain averaged across the seven frequencies is also plotted as the rightmost points in
Figure 6.
Due to the different gain characteristics in velocity and acceleration control (see
Figure 4), two separate 7 (frequency) × 3 (color-feedback type) repeated-measures ANOVAs on the response gain were conducted. For velocity control, both the main effects of frequency and color-feedback type were significant (
F(6, 36) = 13.01,
p < 0.0001 and
F(2, 12) = 5.33,
p = 0.022, respectively). Post hoc Duncun tests revealed that the overall response gain was significantly larger for the position-color (7.18 dB) than for the no-color (6.41 dB,
p = 0.037) or the velocity-color condition (6.14 dB,
p = 0.011), and the overall response gain for the no-color condition was not significantly different from that for the velocity-color condition. For acceleration control, both the main effects of frequency and color-feedback type were also significant (
F(6, 36) = 101.32,
p < 0.0001 and
F(2, 12) = 4.01,
p = 0.046, respectively). Post hoc Duncun tests revealed that the overall response gain was significantly larger for the velocity-color (14.57 dB) than for the no-color (13.82 dB,
p = 0.029) or the position-color condition (13.90 dB,
p = 0.041), and the overall response gain for the no-color condition was not significantly different from that for the position-color condition. Thus, while for velocity control the gains were highest for position-color feedback, for acceleration control the gains were highest for velocity-color feedback. This pattern of results for response gain indicates again that the effect of the added novel color cue on control performance depends on both its relationship to the visual feedback and control dynamics, providing the most benefit when the colored-feedback cue is appropriate for the control dynamics.