There is considerable debate as to whether visual attention is a single unified resource, or whether in fact, judgments of different stimulus attributes (e.g., position, color and luminance) tap into distinct attentional resources. Thus, while several studies have shown comparable levels of interference between similar and dissimilar task pairings (Lee, Itti, Koch, & Braun,
1999; Lee, Koch, & Braun,
1999; Pastukhov, Fischer, & Braun,
2009), Morrone, Denti, and Spinelli (
2002,
2004) report evidence to suggest that color and luminance judgments recruit distinct attentional resources. Parallel debates exist over the extent to which common attentional resources, attentional selection mechanisms (Deubel, Schneider, & Paprotta,
1998; Schneider & Deubel,
2002), and neural representations (Altmann, Grodd, Kourtzi, Bulthoff, & Karnath,
2005; Burr, Morrone, & Ross,
2001; Cavina-Pratesi, Goodale, & Culham,
2007; Collins, Dore-Mazars, & Lappe,
2007; Eckstein, Beutter, Pham, Shimozaki, & Stone,
2007; Eggert, Sailer, Ditterich, & Straube,
2002; Faillenot, Sunaert, Van Hecke, & Orban,
2001; Rice, Valyear, Goodale, Milner, & Culham,
2007; Tibber, Anderson, Melmoth, Rees, & Morgan,
2009; Valyear, Culham, Sharif, Westwood, & Goodale,
2006) subserve perceptual and oculomotor responses (i.e., “vision/selection-for-perception” versus “vision/selection-for-action”). For example, several studies have demonstrated that when eye movements are centrally cued to a peripheral spatial location sensitivity to a perceptual probe is relatively enhanced at the saccadic landing site prior to movement onset (Kowler, Anderson, Dosher, & Blaser,
1995), arguing for a tight and obligatory coupling between eye movements and visuoperceptual attention.