The results of Experiment 1 (
Figure 2) show that the conditions with two and six flankers are very different. With two flankers, there is a drop of performance at the smallest target–flanker distance only (observers LP and JW) or not at all (observer EP). With six flankers, however, there seems to be a quite normal crowding effect that vanishes at near 0.5 E. The results of Experiment 2 (
Figure 3) are qualitatively similar. The present results with two flankers are more or less consistent with Pelli et al.'s (
2004) and Levi et al.'s (
2002) finding that there is no or only a small crowding effect with simple detection and coarse orientation discrimination. However, the idea that feature detection and coarse discrimination are immune to crowding seems to be wrong because the same tasks exhibit normal crowding effects with a larger number of flankers.
With random angular position of flankers, it is possible that the effect of the number of flankers (or a part of it) can be explained by the difference of crowding in the radial vs. tangential direction of the visual field (e.g., Toet & Levi,
1992). When two flankers happen to fall above and below of the target, there should be less crowding (because there are no flankers in the radial direction, where the crowding effect is strongest). With 6 flankers, there is always a pair of flankers near the difficult radial direction. In order to check this possibility, the trials of the two-flanker condition were partitioned in groups based on the angular position of flankers. The comparison of performance for near radial (±30° from horizontal) and near tangential (±30° from vertical) position of flankers is depicted in
Figure 4. The difference is about 8 percentage points across all target–flanker distances. The same effect for different distances is somewhat surprising and suggests that the radial–tangential difference here may not be caused by a usual crowding (which should exhibit a strong effect of target–flanker distance) but by some other mechanism. Anyway, there is no evidence that the effect of two flankers with the radial position could be similar to the effect of six flankers.
Trying to find some hints on mechanisms of crowding, performance was analyzed across trials with different combinations of target and flanker orientations.
Figure 5 shows how the target orientation and the orientations of flankers together determine the observers' responses in the orientation discrimination task. It seems that orientations of flankers have two different effects.
With six flankers, the most salient is an effect of “pooling” (e.g., Parkes, Lund, Angelucci, Solomon, & Morgan,
2001): With increasing number of vertical flankers in a display, the probability of response “vertical” increases, more or less linearly. The linear effect was highly significant for two observers (
F(1, 497) = 11.3,
P = 0.001 and
F(1, 497) = 12.8,
P < 0.001 for EP and JW, respectively) but not significant for LP (
F(1, 397) = 0.94, n. s.). This effect can be explained also by a probabilistic model that selects a single object on each trial, with some preference for the target. With two flankers, this effect is less pronounced (significant for JW (
F(1, 497) = 4.1,
P < 0.05), nearly significant for EP (
F(1, 497) = 3.3,
P = 0.07), not significant for LP (
F(1, 397) = 0.48,
ns).
Another effect is that of homogeneity—the correct responses are more probable when the orientation of (all) the flankers is the same. This effect was consistently significant for the two-flanker condition (
F(2, 494) = 3.7,
P < 0.05;
F(2, 436) = 10.9,
P < 0.001;
F(2, 494) = 5.3,
P = 0.005; for EP, LP, and JW, respectively) and not significant for six flankers. A similar increase of accuracy for homogeneous patterns has been found by Petrov and Popple (
2007) in the full-report task where observers had to identify the orientations of all three Gabor patches in a row presented at 6 deg eccentricity (their Gabors were oriented ±45° from vertical).
For the detection task, however, no systematic effects of the orientation of the flankers were found. It is obvious that the orientation of the flankers cannot bias the detection responses in a way similar to that of the orientation discrimination task. However, there seems to be no such a simple explanation for the absence of the homogeneity effect. Anyway, these differences between the detection and orientation discrimination tasks suggest that the observed pooling and homogeneity effects are not directly related with the effect of number of flankers, because that was similar for the two tasks.