“Low vision” refers to non-correctable low acuity. The causes of low vision are many, but involuntary ocular nystagmus is a frequent secondary effect, and congenital nystagmus (CN) may cause low vision. CN usually involves a slow-phase drift of fixation followed by a quick-phase saccadic return (Abadi,
2002). The direction of eye movement usually is predominantly horizontal, as in the two observers we tested. Temporal frequencies of oscillation typically are in the range of 2–6 Hz, with amplitudes about 2–8° and peak slow-phase velocities often above 100°/s (Abadi & Dickinson,
1986; Abadi et al.,
1999; Abadi & Worfolk,
1989; Clement et al.,
2002; Jacobs & Dell'Osso,
2004; Shallo-Hoffman, Dell'Osso, & Dun,
2004). Reduced contrast sensitivity, orientation discrimination, and acuity of CN patients are believed to reflect cortical mechanisms, perhaps related to amblyopia (Abadi & King-Smith,
1979; Bedell,
2006; Chung & Bedell,
1995; Ukwade, Bedell, & White,
2002). Nystagmus also reduces motion sensitivity (Abadi et al.,
1999; Acheson et al.,
1997). CN usually does not cause oscillopsia (perceived instability of the visual world); but instability is perceived if images are artificially stabilized (Abadi et al.,
1999), implying an extra-retinal negative feedback.
We had previously found high duration thresholds for motion discrimination in observers with CN (Nyquist et al.,
2005). Later pilot studies, however, suggested that low-vision motion sensitivity might be better for faster motions than for the relatively slow speeds (1–2°/s) we used initially. When we examined this in
Experiment 3, thresholds for two low-vision observers were elevated for slow foveal and for peripheral motions, but resembled typically sighted observers for foveal motions for speeds above 1°/s. Saccadic interruptions might hinder sensitivity to slow motions, as the longer presentations may be more vulnerable to saccadic interruption. This possibility, however, does not explain the high peripheral thresholds for these observers.
The interactive effects of CN, speed, and retinal locus warrant further investigation. The finding that low-vision observers have high thresholds for peripheral motion, even at high speeds, may be relevant to visual function. Perhaps the moving visual fields of these observers are perceptually less coherent than those of typically sighted observers. Perhaps these differences in motion threshold are pertinent to suggestions that children with low vision are often less attentive than typically sighted children to the peripheral visual fields (Nyquist,
2007).