Most importantly, we found that holistic processing of individual faces is suddenly disrupted between 60 and 90 degrees This non-linear relationship between orientation and the amount of composite for “same” trials offers a second strong support for the qualitative view of the FIE. This observation is roughly consistent with previous studies that used multiple angles of face rotations by asking viewers to judge the grotesqueness of “thatcherized faces” and reported deviations from linearity at orientations around 90° (e.g., Lewis, 2001; Murray et al.,
2000; Sjoberg & Windes, 1992; Stürzel & Spillmann,
2002). Other experiments aimed at testing the effect of orientation on holistic face processing using tasks such as the categorical perception of faces in noise (McKone et al.,
2001), the perception of a “mooney” face stimulus (McKone,
2004), or the matching of thatcherized faces (Edmonds & Lewis,
2007). However, while these studies generally reported departure from linearity,
1 their data were not as clear-cut as the data reported here because they reported significant differences among adjacent angles of rotation (e.g., between 90° and 120°; see also Jacques & Rossion,
2007). Compared to these previous studies, the present data present several advantages to describe adequately the relationship between the angle of rotation of the face and the holistic face processing, which may account for the categorical effects observed here only. First, we used for the first time the face composite effect, an extremely simple and well-documented measure of holistic face processing in the literature. As illustrated on
Figure 1, it is a strong visual illusion: One cannot prevent seeing the top parts of the faces as being slightly different; they literally fuse with the bottom part to form a whole face percept. In contrast, previous attempts to characterize the orientation function of holistic processing used methods that did not test directly the interdependence of facial features. Rather, these studies increased the difficulty of a face task across multiple orientations by adding noise (McKone et al.,
2001), blurring the faces (Collishaw & Hole,
2002), or presenting the faces in the periphery (McKone,
2004). These methods have all in common the disruption of the diagnosticity of local details (including out of fovea presentation). However, they do not directly measure how much an individual face part influences the perception of another part, as in the face composite effect, which is how holistic processing has been operationalized in the face processing literature (Sergent,
1984; Tanaka & Farah,
1993; Young et al.,
1987). Second, the participants of the present experiment were involved in discriminating faces at the
individual level, not at perceiving the degree of grotesqueness of a face or simply perceiving a face stimulus as a face (Sjoberg & Windes, 1992; Stürzel & Spillman,
2002; Lewis, 2001; McKone,
2004; Murray et al.,
2000, Experiment 2), tasks which may not tap into the encoding of individual of face representations. Thus, the question addressed here was truly how inversion affects holistic processes that contribute to perceiving
individual representation of faces, not the categorization of a face stimulus as a face for instance. Third, these previous studies all used subjective judgment tasks (e.g., ratings or grotesqueness), which lead to a large inter-subject variance rather than a measure of performance and speed during individual face processing, as in the present experiment. Finally, unlike previous studies, we reported both RTs and error rates. This is interesting because the patterns for accuracy and RTs were slightly different (
Figure 4): At 90°, the composite effect is still (non-significantly) larger than at 120° for accuracy, but the opposite is found for RTs, indicating a slight trade-off between the two measures at this angle (and thus an overall equally attenuated effect at these 2 angles).