The data from the second non-motion task, namely the disappearance–appearance task, are shown in red in the plots of
Figure 2 (remember that data on this percept were only collected for SOA levels of 282 ms or greater). It is particularly evident at the longest SOA levels that disappearance and appearance is perceived when the interstimulus interval is longer than those leading to shadow motion and shorter than those leading to optimal motion. The transition point between disappearance–appearance and optimal motion appears to be fairly constant across SOA levels and located somewhat below an ISI of zero, i.e., somewhat above a temporal duty cycle of 0.5, but the transition point between disappearance–appearance and shadow motion is not constant across SOA levels. Instead, it drifts toward more negative ISIs (or, equivalently, toward temporal duty cycles larger than 0.5) as SOA increases. The critical variable for this transition thus appears to be neither ISI nor temporal duty cycle, but the sum of SOA and ISI. This sum, which we shall refer to as the local interstimulus interval (LII), is simply the time interval between stimulus element disappearance and reappearance at the same location, as is illustrated in
Figure 1. In order to make this more readily apparent, the data from the appearance–disappearance task are replotted as a function of LII in
Figure 7. The thick gray line fitted to the data is the product of two cumulative Gaussians
g and
h, which are functions of LII and
δ, respectively, multiplied by the maximal possible rating (5). As is evident in the plots,
g is identical across the SOA levels. The second cumulative Gaussian
h refers to the local temporal duty cycle
δ, which is related to LII by the equation
δ = 1 − LII / (2 SOA). The scale of temporal duty cycles corresponding to the scale of LIIs is shown at the top of each plot. With reference to this upper scale, it can be seen that
h is also identical across the plots. Thus, the curve fitted to the data in all panels of
Figure 7 corresponds to a single function of
δ and LII. Although this function has just four free parameters (
μ and
σ for
g and
h), a rather good fit to the entire data set was obtained, as is evident in
Figure 7. The parameters of the fit are given in
Table 3 for the data pooled across all subjects and for each of the subjects individually. It can be seen that there are some considerable differences between observers, but the data from three of four observers yield estimates of
μ l of about 200 ms and estimates of
μ δ of about 0.6. The basic finding is thus that the tendency to perceive appearance and disappearance depends on two conditions: The local interstimulus interval must exceed a certain critical value, and so must the local temporal duty cycle. Referring back to
Figure 4, the Gaussian
h(
δ) models the categorization between “stimulus motion” and “no stimulus motion” while
g(LII) models the categorization between shadow motion and the disappearance–appearance percepts. Following the scheme in
Figure 4, the Gaussian
f should also have been used to model the data, i.e., we should have used the function
f ×
g ×
h instead of just
g ×
h. However, at the high SOA levels used in the present task,
f can be expected to be effectively equal to 1 based on the flicker data, so that it is irrelevant whether it is multiplied in or not.