The visual flash-lag effect is typically assessed by comparing the apparent relative positions of a brief static flash and a continuously moving target. When both stimuli are physically aligned in space and time, the flash appears to lag behind the position of the moving target, hence the term flash-lag (Mackay,
1958; Nijhawan,
1994).
One of the most popular accounts of the flash-lag is the differential latency hypothesis. The underlying assumption is that different stimuli have different perceptual latencies. Specifically, the theory proposes that the visual system responds with a shorter latency to moving than it does to static stimuli (Baldo & Klein,
1995; Krekelberg & Lappe,
2000; Murakami,
2001; Patel, Ogmen, Bedell, & Sampath,
2000; Purushothaman, Patel, Bedell, & Ogmen,
1998; Whitney & Murakami,
1998; Whitney, Murakami, & Cavanagh,
2000). Accordingly, the apparent offset of flashed and moving stimuli would occur because of a perceptual delay for flashed relative to moving stimuli.
There is scant physiological evidence for a moving stimulus advantage of sufficient magnitude to explain the flash-lag. In fact, it has been shown that neurons in Macaque MT respond more rapidly to transient, as opposed to moving, stimuli (Raiguel, Lagae, Gulyas, & Orban,
1989). This would appear to preclude activity in this region, and presumably its human homologue, from serving as the neural basis for the flash-lag. In contrast, small temporal advantages for moving, over static (or transient), stimuli have been observed in Macaque LGN (Orban, Hoffmann, & Duysens,
1985) and Cat primary visual cortex (Jancke, Erlhagen, Schöner, & Dinse,
2004). However, the extent of these advantages (∼15 ms) is too small to provide a credible explanation for the flash-lag, which has variously been estimated as ∼45–80 ms (Eagleman & Sejnowski,
2000a; Purushothaman et al.,
1998; Whitney & Murakami,
1998).
While there is little physiological evidence for a sufficient temporal advantage for moving stimuli, there is some stronger behavioral support. For instance, manipulations of stimulus speed suggest that the flash-lag is consistent with a uniform temporal delay for static, relative to moving, stimuli (Brenner & Smeets,
2000; Krekelberg & Lappe,
2000; Nijhawan,
1994; but see Wojtach, Sung, Truong, & Purves,
2008). The differential latency hypothesis provides a simple explanation for this. It has also been shown that flash-lag magnitude can be modulated by stimulus-dependent variations in visual latency (Purushothaman et al.,
1998). This too is consistent with the differential latency hypothesis. However, a note of caution should be added to this last point. The fact that flash-lag magnitude can be modulated by latency changes does not dictate that a differential latency normally exists. Rather, the introduction of a latency difference might be an additional fact that modulates an effect that has a quite different underlying cause.
Perhaps the strongest prediction of the differential latency hypothesis is that moving stimuli should have a temporal advantage that results in them being seen before coincident static stimuli. Eagleman and Sejnowski (
2000b) attempted to test this. They had observers judge the timing of a flash relative to the cessation of movement. They found no evidence for a temporal advantage for either stimulus event, which is seemingly inconsistent with the differential latency hypothesis. However, this challenge was somewhat undermined by the fact that there can be no flash-lag effect at the offset of a moving stimulus (Eagleman & Sejnowski,
2000a).
To test this central prediction of the differential latency hypothesis more directly, this study will use temporal judgments concerning continuously moving stimuli. Specifically, observers will judge the relative timings of color changes in continuously moving and static stimulus sections. In another condition, observers will judge the relative alignment of moving and static stimulus sections at the time of a color change.
We reason that if the flash-lag is driven by a simple differential latency, that causes moving stimuli to be seen before static stimuli, all perceptual decisions regarding moving stimuli should show a temporal advantage relative to static stimuli. In our experiments, this should include both our relative timing and alignment judgments. However, if the flash-lag is driven by processes that selectively impact spatial judgments, we would not expect a temporal advantage for moving stimuli when judging apparent timing.