Disparity acuity reaches less than 5 arcsec under optimal conditions (Andersen & Weymouth,
1923; Berry,
1948; Westheimer & McKee,
1978)—less than the width of a single cone (Curcio,
1987). In constructing a neuron's receptive field, the visual system cannot control both the positions of the cone photoreceptors from which the neuron draws signals and the types of cones (L, M, or S) from which it draws signals: High-precision spatial sampling is purchased at the expense of high-precision chromatic sampling and vice versa. This problem is compounded by the random arrangement of cone photoreceptors of different types in the retinal mosaic (Roorda, Metha, Lennie, & Williams,
2001), which gives rise to clusters of cones of a single type. Clusters in the two eyes will be aligned only by chance, so it might be especially difficult to provide a binocularly driven neuron with both the high-precision spatial sampling required for computing stereoscopic depth and the high-precision chromatic sampling required for encoding chromaticity. Frequent findings of poor chromatic stereopsis (e.g. de Weert,
1979; Kingdom & Simmons,
1996; Krauskopf & Forte,
2002) suggest that the brain might have resolved this problem by confining chromatic analysis to monocular pathways (Hubel & Livingstone,
1987), although some studies suggest that isoluminant stereopsis remains possible under ideal conditions (Grinberg & Williams,
1985; Kingdom & Simmons,
1996).
We test this directly by characterizing the tuning of V1 and V2 neurons in the macaque for chromatic stimulation in both eyes. We show that color-preferring cells are often driven well through both eyes, and further that in these cells the chromatic properties of the receptive fields are well matched, so they are equipped to provide a binocular representation of color. The receptive fields of color-preferring cells generally show little selectivity for spatial form (Hubel & Wiesel,
1968; Johnson, Hawken, & Shapley,
2001; Lennie, Krauskopf, & Sclar,
1990; Solomon, Peirce, & Lennie,
2004), and as a result they are poorly suited for coding binocular depth, which must involve precise comparisons of spatial signals from the eyes (Anzai, Ohzawa, & Freeman,
1999; Ohzawa, DeAngelis, & Freeman,
1990). The receptive fields of binocular neurons that responded well to both color and luminance usually show greater spatial selectivity, but the chromatic properties of their receptive fields are less well matched, and they are therefore poorly suited for coding binocular color.