Our perception of motion relies upon a largely hierarchical process, with the initial extraction of motion performed by spatially restricted local-motion detectors, prior to extensive spatial integration within the global-motion stage (e.g., Braddick,
1997; Movshon, Adelson, Gizzi, & Newsome,
1986; Welch,
1989). Though much is known about the global-motion stage, relatively little attention has been directed toward the number of global directions that can be detected simultaneously. This capacity sets an important upper limit for the integration and segmentation operations that characterize global-motion processing (Braddick,
1993), as well as the read-out algorithms used to interpret population activity within this stage (Pouget, Dayan, & Zemel,
2000). Recently, a strict capacity limit has been found for the detection of transparent motion, which occurs when multiple objects move simultaneously through the same spatial region. When simulated with random-dot stimuli, where multiple interspersed dot groups move in distinct directions (e.g., Clarke,
1977), no more than two directions can be detected simultaneously (Edwards & Greenwood,
2005), with the addition of speed or disparity differences able to extend this to three (Greenwood & Edwards,
2006a,
2006b). However, though the representation of transparency depends heavily on the signal-to-noise operations of the global-motion stage (e.g., Edwards & Greenwood,
2005; Snowden & Verstraten,
1999), signal intensities within these stimuli should have allowed the detection of up to four directions. The aim of the present study was thus to determine whether this three-signal limit reflects the specific mechanisms of transparent-motion detection, or a more general restriction on the detection of multiple global-motion signals.