October 2003
Volume 3, Issue 9
Free
Vision Sciences Society Annual Meeting Abstract  |   October 2003
Motion-biasing, not asynchronous feature binding, explains the feature-flash drag effect
Author Affiliations
  • David M Eagleman
    The Salk Institute, CA, USA
Journal of Vision October 2003, Vol.3, 189. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/3.9.189
  • Views
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      David M Eagleman, Terrence J Sejnowski; Motion-biasing, not asynchronous feature binding, explains the feature-flash drag effect. Journal of Vision 2003;3(9):189. https://doi.org/10.1167/3.9.189.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Cai and Schlag [VSS 2001] demonstrated that a flashed feature of a moving object — say, a translating bar that flashes blue for one frame — is mislocalized to a later point in the trajectory. If the bar changes height as it moves, then the blueness appears to fill the height of a bar further along the trajectory. They suggested the illusion results from asynchronous feature binding (AFB), i.e., the flashed color change is “delayed and assigned to a later occurring bar” [Cai & Cavanagh, VSS 2002]. Our evidence speaks against an AFB explanation, suggesting instead that motion signals drag the position judgments of the feature-flashed bar (causing “feature-flash drag’). To distinguish between the AFB and motion-biasing models, we presented the illusion with fewer apparent-motion stations: the bar completes its trajectory in the same amount of time, but with fewer appearances. Here, the blue bar appears shifted to a location where no bar was presented. This speaks against AFB, which predicts the assignment of blue to a later appearance of the bar. Cai & Cavanagh suggested that positions of the moving object may be interpolated before AFB; however, our alternative hypothesis of motion-biasing may account for more data (ref. Whitney, 2002). Next, Ss counted the number of white bars that appeared after the blue bar. In the AFB model, blueness assigned to a later bar should cause Ss to undercount. However, Ss counted veridically, supporting the motion-biasing model. Lastly, the blue bar is seen at intermediate heights that never actually occurred (also shown by Cai & Cavanagh). We suggest that motion signals may be applied differentially to the individual edges of an object (here, the top and bottom edges of a growing bar move orthogonally to the bar's translation). Instead of asynchronous perception, this illusion may demonstrate the generality of motion biasing and offer a unified explanation for flash-lag and flash-drag effects.

Eagleman, D. M., Sejnowski, T. J.(2003). Motion-biasing, not asynchronous feature binding, explains the feature-flash drag effect [Abstract]. Journal of Vision, 3( 9): 189, 189a, http://journalofvision.org/3/9/189/, doi:10.1167/3.9.189. [CrossRef]
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×