July 2013
Volume 13, Issue 9
Free
Vision Sciences Society Annual Meeting Abstract  |   July 2013
Winner-take-all circuits exhibit key hallmarks of binocular rivalry
Author Affiliations
  • Svenja Marx
    Neurophysics, Philipps-University Marburg, Germany
  • Gina Gruenhage
    Neurophysics, Philipps-University Marburg, Germany\nBernstein Center for Computational Neurosciences, Berlin, Germany
  • Daniel Walper
    Neurophysics, Philipps-University Marburg, Germany
  • Ueli Rutishauser
    Neurosurgery, Cedars-Sinai Medical Center, Los Angeles, CA, USA\nDivision of Biology, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA, USA
  • Wolfgang Einhauser
    Neurophysics, Philipps-University Marburg, Germany\nCenter for Interdisciplinary Research (ZiF), Bielefeld, Germany
Journal of Vision July 2013, Vol.13, 534. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/13.9.534
  • Views
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Svenja Marx, Gina Gruenhage, Daniel Walper, Ueli Rutishauser, Wolfgang Einhauser; Winner-take-all circuits exhibit key hallmarks of binocular rivalry. Journal of Vision 2013;13(9):534. https://doi.org/10.1167/13.9.534.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Perception is inherently ambiguous. Binocular and perceptual rivalry model such ambiguity by presenting constant stimuli that evoke alternating perceptual interpretations. We modeled key phenomena that are common to nearly all forms of rivalry: i) Dominance durations, the times in which a single percept is perceived, follow a heavy-tailed distribution. ii) Changes in stimulus strength (e.g., contrast of one or both stimuli) have well defined effects on dominance durations (Levelt's propositions). iii) Sufficiently long periodic stimulus removal ("blanking") stabilizes the percept, while short blanking destabilizes it. Our model consisted of three coupled winner-take-all (WTA) circuits. Each WTA circuit contained two excitatory units that projected to one inhibitory unit, which in turn projected to both excitatory units. To test the model's quantitative predictions, we performed two binocular rivalry experiments for obtaining fine-grained measurements about the relation between stimulus strength and dominance durations. Small (0.6° visual angle) sinusoidal gratings (oriented +45° in one eye, -45° in the other) were used at 6 contrast levels per eye (36 in total). Contrast levels were adjusted individually. In experiment 1, stimuli were continuously presented (blank duration 0); in experiment 2 the number of contrast levels was reduced to 3, but blank duration and inter-blank presentation duration were varied. We found that the network exhibited all three aforementioned hallmarks of rivalry. Furthermore, the model made novel predictions on the functional dependence of dominance durations on stimulus strength, blank duration and inter-blank duration, which were well compatible with experimental data. Beyond predicting all hallmarks of rivalry, our model is well founded in neuronal circuitry. It is a generic model of competitive processes rather than tailored to explain specific aspects of rivalry. Hence our model provides a natural link from rivalry to other forms of perceptual ambiguity and to other competitive processes, such as attention and decision-making.

Meeting abstract presented at VSS 2013

×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×