The duration of each fixation was calculated from the video transcriptions. The frequency distributions are shown in
Figures 4 and
5. Data for the three subjects in
Figure 4 were recorded with the image of the eye provided by the tracker, superimposed on the record from the scene camera. This allowed careful monitoring of the fixation durations measurements, since a transient track loss sometimes results in a deviation of the cursor position, and thus appears like the termination of a fixation. Movement of the eye during the track loss could be observed directly in the eye image. The distributions are quite similar for the different subjects. The data for the seven subjects in
Figure 5 did not have the eye image available on the video record. It is therefore possible that these data are partially contaminated by transient track losses. This should not be a major factor, however, as segments of the tape where the cursor disappeared, indicating a track loss, were eliminated from the analysis. The most distinctive feature of these distributions is their wide spread. Fixations range from under 100 msec to over 1500 msec. There is some variation between subjects, but most of the distributions have a mode between 100 and 200 msec, which is less than for reading or picture viewing (
Henderson & Hollingworth, 1999). The very long fixations are usually associated with some prolonged action of the hands that required continuous guidance, such as spreading, scooping out peanut butter, pouring, or undoing the tie on the bread bag. Land et al (
1999) observed a similarly wide spread of fixation durations in their tea-making task. For the long fixations, it is important to note that the noise in the tracker made it impossible to identify small saccades within a radius of about 1.5° around fixation. If these were present, the number of long fixations would be overestimated. Although it is impossible to know what the role of individual fixations is from such observations, it appears that, to a first approximation, the fixation durations are determined by the momentary task demands. Gaze often departs just at the point a hand movement is complete, or there is no longer need for visual guidance. An example of this is given in
Figure 3, and in the accompanying video, where the eye departed from controlling knife placement when the knife was close to the plate, and the remainder of the movement could be controlled using somatosensory information. The eye then arrived to guide lid pickup just as the left hand approached the lid. Similar time-locking of fixations to critical stages of the actions was observed by Johansson et al (
2001). This is, of course, an incomplete description of determinants of fixation duration. In a number of instances vision may not be providing critical information for the ongoing action. For example, screwing the cap on the soda bottle can be completed under proprioceptive control and it is not obvious what role is being played by fixation during such periods.