Despite the promise demonstrated in the current study, the
qCSF has several shortcomings. (1)
It uses a forced-choice task with a high-guessing rate. The possibility of improving the test's efficiency by using a
Yes–No task is tempered by the introduction of unconstrained response criteria (Klein,
2001). One potential approach to address the response bias confound is to rapidly estimate the response bias in
YN tasks directly (Lesmes, Lu et al.,
2006) or add a rated response to the forced-choice task (Kaernbach,
2001; Klein,
2001). (2)
The spatial CSF is limited as characteristic of spatiotemporal vision. Because CSF shape depends on factors that include temporal frequency (Kelly,
1979; van Nes et al.,
1967), spatial and temporal envelopes (Peli, Arend, Young, & Goldstein,
1993), and retinal illuminance (Koenderink et al.,
1978c), clarifying the best
qCSF clinical testing conditions requires measuring the spatiotemporal contrast sensitivity surface (Kelly,
1979), which describes contrast thresholds as a function of spatial and temporal frequencies. The practical difficulty of measuring contrast sensitivity across this 2-D surface typically focuses investigation to only one of its cross-sections: (a) a spatial CSF at constant temporal frequency (Campbell & Robson,
1968), (b) a temporal CSF at constant spatial frequency (de Lange,
1958), or (c) a constant-speed CSF at co-varying spatial and temporal frequencies (Kelly,
1979). To improve measurements of spatiotemporal vision, we have developed the
quick Surface (or
qSurface) method (Lesmes, Gepshtein, Lu, & Albright,
2009), which leverages multiple
qCSF applications to estimate different cross-sections of the spatiotemporal contrast sensitivity surface in parallel. Whereas the
qCSF method evaluates stimuli for their contributions to a single cross-section through the spatiotemporal sensitivity surface, the
qSurface method evaluates grating stimuli (defined by contrast and spatial and temporal frequencies) for the information they provide about concurrent estimates of horizontal, vertical, and diagonal cross-sections through the surface. This innovation greatly reduces the testing time for estimating the spatiotemporal sensitivity surface and even allows for the measurement of multiple surfaces in an experimental session (≈1 h). As a result, the
qSurface method should be a valuable tool for finding the most useful spatiotemporal condition(s) for CSF clinical testing and for studying spatiotemporal vision in general. (3)
The functional form used by the qCSF cannot accommodate notches or other local deficits. To address this shortcoming, we are currently developing adaptive CSF procedures with fewer model-based assumptions. These tests will be more flexible to detect aberrant CSF features with potential clinical importance (Tahir, Parry, Pallikaris, & Murray,
2009; Woods, Bradley, & Atchison,
1996). Forthcoming work that addresses the above-mentioned shortcomings, in combination with inevitable increases in computing power, should ultimately improve the efficiency of the next generation of
qCSF methods.