Surface occlusion that occurs frequently in the external visual scene presents a special challenge to the surface representation process. A partially occluded surface does not have its occluded parts imaged on the retina, while the same surface's non-occluded parts are imaged on the retina as separated image patches. Thus, to represent a partially occluded surface, the visual system has to fill-in the occluded parts of the surface and piece them together with the separated, non-occluded parts of the surface (amodal surface completion). Broadly speaking, the visual system can use two classes of cues to construct the occluded surface. The first is related to the surface relationship
between the occluding and occluded surfaces, e.g., the various contour junctions, relative motion parallax, relative binocular disparity, etc. The second class of cues is related to the surface relationship
between the non-occluded parts of the surface, which includes their geometric relationship and surface feature similarity. The geometric relationship pertains to, for example, whether the visible (non-occluded) surface patches have similar surface curvature, or whether they are aligned and could potentially form a smooth continuous surface (e.g., Fantoni, Bertamini, & Gerbino,
2005; Kanizsa,
1979; Kellman, Garrigan, & Shipley,
2005; Kellman & Shipley,
1991; Nakayama & Shimojo,
1990; Nakayama, Shimojo, & Silverman,
1989; Sekuler,
1994; Tse,
1999). The surface feature cue is concerned with whether the non-occluded surface patches have similar surface feature properties such as texture, color, luminance, etc. (He & Ooi,
1998; Kanizsa,
1979; Koffka,
1935; Spehar,
2000; Spehar & Clifford,
2003; Yin, Kellman, & Shipley,
1997,
2000). Whereas much research has shown elements with similar surface feature properties tend to group together, few studies have demonstrated this Gestalt principle (similarity) applies to amodal surface integration (e.g., Yin et al.,
1997,
2000). Of the few such studies, Yin et al. (
1997) used a display where an opaque rectangle occludes the middle section of a curved bar. A small disc was added onto the rectangular surface and located along the invisible path of the occluded curved bar. The authors found that when the surface feature (texture and color) of the disc was similar to the features of the visible parts of the partially occluded bar, observers were more likely to perceive the disc as an aperture (hole). In contrast, their observers had a bias to perceive the disc as a figure (spot) on the rectangular surface when it had different surface features from the partially occluded bar. Since the observed completion was largely based on surface features, and because the boundary contour of the disc could not integrate with the boundary contour of the occluded bar, Yin et al. coined this “surface completion”. Yet, the general lack of empirical evidence of surface features contributing to surface contour completion has led most theories of surface representation to assign a modest role to surface features in the surface completion process (Grossberg & Mingolla,
1985; Kellman & Shipley,
1991). Given this, a goal of our paper is to investigate whether luminance contrast polarity, a fundamental surface feature property, plays a significant role in representing a partially occluded surface.