Situations in which conscious visual perception can change despite consistent retinal input provide fascinating opportunities to explore the nature of visual awareness (Alais & Blake,
2005; Arnold, Grove, & Wallis,
2007; Blake & Logothetis,
2002; Crick,
1994; Kim & Blake,
2005; Leopold & Logothetis,
1996,
1999; Logothetis & Schall,
1989). One example of such a situation is motion-induced blindness (MIB; Bonneh, Cooperman, & Sagi,
2001). This remarkable phenomenon is characterized by intermittent perceptual disappearances of usually salient visual targets in the presence of a superimposed moving dot display. In this situation, when the targets disappear, they can remain suppressed for several seconds.
Several explanations for MIB have been proposed. Bonneh et al. (
2001) have suggested that MIB disappearances are caused by attentional competition between different display elements. Graf, Adams, and Lages (
2002) have shown that disappearances are strongly modulated by occlusion cues. A perceptual filling-in mechanism (Hsu, Yeh, & Kramer,
2004,
2006), adaptation of target borders (Hsu et al.,
2006; Kawabe & Miura,
2007), interhemispheric switching (Carter & Pettigrew,
2003; Funk & Pettigrew,
2003), and more recently gain control reduction accompanied by a response bias shift (Caetta, Gorea, & Bonneh,
2007) have also been implicated. However, the role of motion itself has not been addressed in detail by any of these accounts.
In their original paper, Bonneh et al. (
2001) manipulated the speed of different types of dot masks (a kinetic depth sphere, a 2D rotating surface of crosses, and a 1D translating cross surface). They found that disappearance proportions increased with increasing mask speeds. However, these stimuli all contained broadband spatial and temporal information that makes Bonneh et al.'s findings regarding speed impossible to disentangle from the plausible influence of temporal frequency.
Kawabe and Miura (
2007) have recently shown that temporal luminance modulations (flicker) are sufficient to induce subjective MIB-like disappearances. Specifically, they showed that a ring surrounding the MIB target produced significantly more disappearances when it was flickered (at 9.38 Hz) compared to when it was static. These disappearances were greatest when the ring was near, but not overlapping, the target (Kawabe & Miura,
2007). Kawabe and Miura's findings suggest that the critical determinant of MIB might be temporal modulation rather than retinal motion per se.
However, at present the link between flicker-induced blindness (FIB; Kawabe & Miura,
2007) and MIB (Bonneh at al.,
2001) is unclear, and it is at least possible that the two are unrelated. For instance, flicker-induced disappearances could be a consequence of some form of temporal masking (Anderson & Burr,
1985; Cass & Alais,
2006; Hess & Snowden,
1992; Snowden & Hess,
1992), whereas MIB might be a consequence of some combination of the aforementioned MIB explanations. The suggestion that a common mechanism underlies both phenomena (Kawabe & Miura,
2007) would be strengthened if their temporal tuning characteristics were found to be similar.
To assess this possibility, the present study will disambiguate the roles of temporal luminance modulation and retinal stimulus speed. Speed is a product of both the temporal frequency (TF) and the spatial frequency (SF) of a stimulus. If disappearances are sensitive to retinal speed, the TF at which disappearances are greatest will vary as SF changes. Alternatively, if disappearances are insensitive to retinal stimulus speed, the temporal frequency at which disappearances are greatest will not vary even if SF is changed.